2005
DOI: 10.1080/0272499044000032
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Perception of Empty and Filled Time Intervals by Pigeons

Abstract: Pigeons were trained in a within-subjects design to discriminate durations of a filled interval (2 s and 8 s of light) and durations of an empty interval (2 s and 8 s bound by two 500-ms light markers). Filled intervals required a response to one set of comparisons (e.g., blue vs. yellow), whereas empty intervals required a response to a different set of comparisons (e.g., red vs. green). Psychophysical testing indicated that empty intervals were judged to be longer than equivalent durations of a filled interv… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
17
0

Year Published

2005
2005
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 48 publications
5
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition, the empty-filled difference in timing should be greater for psychophysical tests conducted with the 4-versus 16-sec anchor durations than for the 2-versus 8-sec anchor durations. This will replicate an important previous result (Miki & Santi, 2005) indicating that dif-f f ferent sets of anchor durations produce a multiplicative timing difference rather than an additive timing difference. Additive timing differences are typically attributed to timing latency effects, whereas multiplicative timing differences are attributed to clock-rate differences (see Penney, Gibbon, & Meck, 2000;Wearden, Edwards, Fakhri, & Percival, 1998).…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 84%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In addition, the empty-filled difference in timing should be greater for psychophysical tests conducted with the 4-versus 16-sec anchor durations than for the 2-versus 8-sec anchor durations. This will replicate an important previous result (Miki & Santi, 2005) indicating that dif-f f ferent sets of anchor durations produce a multiplicative timing difference rather than an additive timing difference. Additive timing differences are typically attributed to timing latency effects, whereas multiplicative timing differences are attributed to clock-rate differences (see Penney, Gibbon, & Meck, 2000;Wearden, Edwards, Fakhri, & Percival, 1998).…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 84%
“…Only recently has this topic been systematically investigated in animals. Miki and Santi (2005) trained pigeons in a within-subjects design to discriminate empty intervals (short or long durations bound by two 500-msec light markers) and filled intervals (short or long durations of a continuous visual signal). Empty intervals required a response to one set of colored comparisons, and filled intervals required a response to a different set of colored comparisons.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because of the large number of highly ambiguous results, in his comp prehensive reviews of the influence of filled and empty intervals on performance on duration discrimination in humans, Grondin (2001Grondin ( , 2003 arrived at the conclusion that, to date, no definitive statement on this issue can be made. Most interestingly, similarly inconclusive results have been reported from animal studies (e.g., Kraemer, Randall, & Brown, 1997;MacInnis, 2007;Miki & Santi, 2005;Santi, Keough, Gagne, & van Rooyen, 2007;Santi, Miki, Hornyak, & Eidse, 2006). At this point, it seems that differences in duration discrimination with filled and empty intervals may depend on various factors, such as the duration of the intervals to be compared, the sensory y p , p y modality in which the intervals are presented, the physical characteristics of the markers defining the empty intervals, or the psychophysical procedure applied (cf.…”
mentioning
confidence: 74%
“…These criteria excluded (see Figure 1): (1) experiments using time judgment durations shorter than 1 second (e.g., Aubry, Guillaume, Mogicato, Bergeret, & Celsis, 2008), animals (e.g., Miki & Santi, 2005), participants exhibiting psychopathology (e.g., Rueda & SchmitterEdgecombe, 2009), participants experiencing an unusual physical condition (e.g., Cheng, Ali, & Meck, 2007, or those in an altered state of consciousness (e.g., Bowers & Brenneman, 1979); (2) studies that used bisection or temporal generalisation methods, or experiments in which participants made qualitative judgments (e.g., Grondin & McAuley, 2009); (3) studies that did not report an adequate level of statistics to compute the effects sizes or the the subjective/objective ratio (e.g., Block & Reed, 1978); and, (4) studies in which researchers did not obtain and report a perceived time dilation(e.g., Hellsrtom & Carlsson, 1997) (see Figure 1). …”
Section: Sample Of Studiesmentioning
confidence: 99%