2007
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00581.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The informative value of emotional expressions: ‘social referencing’ in mother–child pretense

Abstract: Mothers begin to pretend with their children during the second year, when children still have much to learn about the real world. Although it would be easy to confuse what is pretend with what is real, children at this young age often demonstrate comprehension during pretense situations. It is plausible that social referencing, in which the child uses the mother's emotional expression as a guide to behavior, might facilitate this emerging knowledge by signaling to the child not to take the pretend situation se… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

0
23
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 61 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 11 publications
0
23
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Furthermore, Gardner et al (1988) suggested it is likely that 4-year-olds understand that emotions expressed in pretend play do not need to correspond to internal emotions. Indeed, a number of pretend play features could indicate that children may understand the emotional AR distinction better in pretend play than in deception situations: (a) the existence of the mannerisms, or the typical gestures of pretend play which help children to distinguish when someone is pretending (Leslie, 1988;Lillard & Witherington, 2004;Piaget, 1962); (b) the specificity of the language used during pretend play (Auwarter, 1986;Garvey & Kramer, 1989); (c) the evidence that social referencing might function as a sign for the children to interpret the pretence acts (Nishida & Lillard, 2007); (d) children's early capacity to distinguish reality from fiction (Giménez-Dasí, 2003); and (e) the fact that while in deception acts there is always someone who knows something about the real state of the affairs and another person who does not, in pretence, all participants share the same representation of the reality (Peskin, 1996). Therefore, we expected children in the present research to understand the emotional AR distinction before and better in pretend play situations than in deception situations.…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Furthermore, Gardner et al (1988) suggested it is likely that 4-year-olds understand that emotions expressed in pretend play do not need to correspond to internal emotions. Indeed, a number of pretend play features could indicate that children may understand the emotional AR distinction better in pretend play than in deception situations: (a) the existence of the mannerisms, or the typical gestures of pretend play which help children to distinguish when someone is pretending (Leslie, 1988;Lillard & Witherington, 2004;Piaget, 1962); (b) the specificity of the language used during pretend play (Auwarter, 1986;Garvey & Kramer, 1989); (c) the evidence that social referencing might function as a sign for the children to interpret the pretence acts (Nishida & Lillard, 2007); (d) children's early capacity to distinguish reality from fiction (Giménez-Dasí, 2003); and (e) the fact that while in deception acts there is always someone who knows something about the real state of the affairs and another person who does not, in pretence, all participants share the same representation of the reality (Peskin, 1996). Therefore, we expected children in the present research to understand the emotional AR distinction before and better in pretend play situations than in deception situations.…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Most preschoolers have a more action-based understanding of pretending, in which they equate pretending with behaving like or appearing like an entity, regardless of that person's other mental states (Lillard, 1993b(Lillard, , 1996(Lillard, , 1998Sobel, 2004Sobel, , 2007Sobel & Lillard, 2002), even though they can pretend appropriately (e.g., Harris & Kavanaugh, 1993) and recognize when another person is pretending just from behavioral cues (e.g., Lillard & Witherington, 2004;Ma & Lillard, 2006;Nishida & Lillard, 2007). By age 6 to 7 years old, the majority of children begin to respond based on the individual's mental states (Richert & Lillard, 2002).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In accord with studies on this phenomenon (Nishida & Lillard, 2007; Sorce et al, 1985; Vaish & Striano, 2004), social looking was defined as infants' looks towards the parent. If infants were smiling during the look, then this behavior was coded as “baby smiles at parent” instead of social looking.…”
Section: 1 Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, Nishida and Lillard (2007) point out that SR research employs strange or confusing situations (e.g., visual cliff, confronting a stranger), and propose that researchers should investigate whether SR is used in “situations that are not entirely novel and ambiguous, but slightly ‘out-of-the-ordinary’” (p.206). For example, although numerous studies have shown that infants use SR to interpret ambiguity as threatening, none have examined whether they use it to interpret ambiguity as humorous.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation