2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.appdev.2015.02.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Socioeconomic status (SES) affects means–end behavior across the first year

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
11
0
4

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
0
11
0
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Under this explanation, an infant's ability to process visual information efficiently could overcome other delays or deficiencies often reported in infants of low maternal education. For example, mothers with low maternal education have infants who have delays with attention (Clearfield & Jedd, 2013), object exploration (Clearfield, Bailey, Jenne, Stanger & Tacke, 2014;Tacke, Bailey & Clearfield, 2015), cognitive flexibility (Clearfield & Niman, 2012;Lipina, Martelli, Vuelta & Colombo, 2005) and means-ends behaviour (Clearfield, Stanger & Jenne, 2015).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Under this explanation, an infant's ability to process visual information efficiently could overcome other delays or deficiencies often reported in infants of low maternal education. For example, mothers with low maternal education have infants who have delays with attention (Clearfield & Jedd, 2013), object exploration (Clearfield, Bailey, Jenne, Stanger & Tacke, 2014;Tacke, Bailey & Clearfield, 2015), cognitive flexibility (Clearfield & Niman, 2012;Lipina, Martelli, Vuelta & Colombo, 2005) and means-ends behaviour (Clearfield, Stanger & Jenne, 2015).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Traditionally, successful means-end behaviors have been reported to emerge at about 5-6 months of age for less complex tasks, such as selecting the correct lever and visually attending to the resulting activation of a distant toy (Lobo & Galloway, 2008). By 7-8 months, infants begin solving means-end problems requiring one intermediate step, such as pulling a support to retrieve a distant toy (Clearfield, Stanger, & Jenne, 2015;Munakata et al, 1997;Willatts, 1984aWillatts, , 1985Willatts, , 1999. At 9 months, infants are capable of solving two-step problems, such as removing an obstructing barrier and then retrieving an object by pulling a support (Willatts, 1984a).…”
Section: Development Of Means-end Problem Solvingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Intentionality is the deliberate or purposeful projection of awareness toward some object or outcome (Schlitz, 1996). Means-end problemsolving tasks can serve as effective tools for assessing intentionality in infants (Clearfield et al, 2015;Piaget, 1953;Schlesinger & Langer, 1999;Uzgiris & Hunt, 1975;Willatts, 1984aWillatts, , 1999. Piaget (1953Piaget ( , 1955 suggested that 4-8-month-old infants lack the concept of causality, do not distinguish means from end, and may only accidentally solve a means-end task.…”
Section: Emergence Of Intentionality In Means-end Problem Solvingmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Existem evidências consistentes a respeito do impacto de variáveis sociodemográficas na interação mãe-bebê e no desenvolvimento da criança. Bebês de famílias socioeconomicamente vulneráveis, aquelas que possuem menor poder aquisitivo e menores níveis de escolaridade, tendem a apresentar atrasos no desenvolvimento cognitivo que podem ser constatados desde os primeiros meses de vida (Clearfield, Stanger, & Jenne, 2015;Koutra et al, 2013;Mermelshtine & Barnes, 2016). De modo mais específico, a escolaridade dos pais, a coabitação parental e a renda familiar têm sido relacionadas ao desenvolvimento cognitivo e motor.…”
Section: Introductionunclassified