2014
DOI: 10.1017/s0272263114000321
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Mood Selection in Relative Clauses

Abstract: There is presently a lively debate in second language (L2) acquisition research as to whether (adult) learners can acquire linguistic phenomena located at the interface between syntax and other modules, such as semantics, pragmatics, and lexical semantics, in contrast to phenomena that are purely syntactic in nature. For some researchers, the interface is precisely the place where fossilization occurs and the source of nonconvergence in L2 speakers. In this article we focus on the acquisition of the morphosynt… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

3
24
0
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
3
24
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In isolation, the use of subjunctive mood in an ARC, according to Borgonovo et al (2015), results in an attributive reading, implying, in this case, that the speaker does not presuppose the existence of the trilingual robot in question. The use of the indicative mood, on the other hand, triggers what Borgonovo et al have called a referential reading, signaling that the speaker does, in fact, presuppose the existence of a particular trilingual robot (e.g., his own trilingual robot who tends to get lost a lot.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In isolation, the use of subjunctive mood in an ARC, according to Borgonovo et al (2015), results in an attributive reading, implying, in this case, that the speaker does not presuppose the existence of the trilingual robot in question. The use of the indicative mood, on the other hand, triggers what Borgonovo et al have called a referential reading, signaling that the speaker does, in fact, presuppose the existence of a particular trilingual robot (e.g., his own trilingual robot who tends to get lost a lot.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Despite the extensive instruction L2 speakers receive on the subjunctive, it remains a vulnerable area for this population, especially in those contexts where mood is variable and determined by semantic and pragmatic features (Iverson et al 2008;Borgonovo et al 2015). Similar problems are reported for heritage speakers of Spanish e.g., (Silva-Corvalán 1994;Montrul 2009).…”
Section: The Subjunctivementioning
confidence: 76%
“…In example (2), if the speaker refers to a specific shirt, indicative is more appropriate, but if he is looking for any shirt (as long as it has big buttons), subjunctive is more felicitous. Given that the specificity (a semantic feature) of the antecedent is involved, this use of the subjunctive pertains to the interface between syntax and semantics following (Borgonovo et al 2015).…”
Section: The Subjunctivementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The contrast between the indicative and subjunctive moods in Spanish relative clauses has garnered significant attention in the fields of First Language (L1) (Blake 1983; Pérez-Leroux 1998), Second Language (L2) (Borgonovo et al 2015), and Heritage Language (HL) (Montrul 2007; Montrul and Perpiñán 2011) Acquisition. With respect to L1, it has been found that, even though children acquire Spanish subjunctive morphology early in the language acquisition process, they only master mood selection in relative clauses around the age of 12 (Blake 1983), showing a correlation between cognitive and linguistic development (Pérez-Leroux 1998).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%