2019
DOI: 10.3390/languages4030069
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Late(r) Bird Gets the Verb? Effects of Age of Acquisition of English on Adult Heritage Speakers’ Knowledge of Subjunctive Mood in Spanish

Abstract: Many previous studies have found that adult heritage speakers exhibit significant variability in their production and comprehension of mood morphology in Spanish. Nonetheless, it remains unclear what specific factors predict heritage speakers’ likelihood of exhibiting such variability. The present study contributes to this question by testing the effect of both (a) age-of-acquisition of English and (b) Spanish proficiency on heritage speakers’ productive and receptive knowledge of mood morphology. Seventeen “e… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
8
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
(57 reference statements)
1
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Second, all HSs were “second generation” (Portes & Rumbaut, 2014), meaning that at least one of their parents immigrated to the United States from a Spanish‐speaking country at age 13 or later. The decision to use this age cut‐off, rather than 17 years (e.g., Otheguy & Zentella, 2012), was based on previous studies showing that Spanish‐speakers who immigrate at age 13 produce the subjunctive categorically in obligatory subjunctive conditions, even after learning English and spending multiple years in the United States (Giancaspro, 2019a, 2019b, 2020; Perez‐Cortes, 2016). In our sample, all but five HSs (12%) had two first‐generation parents.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Second, all HSs were “second generation” (Portes & Rumbaut, 2014), meaning that at least one of their parents immigrated to the United States from a Spanish‐speaking country at age 13 or later. The decision to use this age cut‐off, rather than 17 years (e.g., Otheguy & Zentella, 2012), was based on previous studies showing that Spanish‐speakers who immigrate at age 13 produce the subjunctive categorically in obligatory subjunctive conditions, even after learning English and spending multiple years in the United States (Giancaspro, 2019a, 2019b, 2020; Perez‐Cortes, 2016). In our sample, all but five HSs (12%) had two first‐generation parents.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because previous studies (Giancaspro, 2019a, 2019b; Montrul, 2009; Montrul & Perpiñán, 2011; Perez‐Cortes, 2016, 2021) have found proficiency to be a significant predictor of HS variability, we expect HSs with lower HL proficiency to exhibit more variability than higher proficiency HSs. Furthermore, in line with the investigations summarized above (Giancaspro, 2020; Perez‐Cortes, 2022; Poplack et al., 2013; inter alia), we also hypothesize that HSs will be more likely to perform variably with regular verbs—which are less autonomous (Bybee & Brewer, 1980) and less salient (Polinsky, 2018)—than with irregular verbs, which are more autonomous and more salient.…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The US variety of Spanish has been the subject of a myriad of linguistic studies over the past several decades, ranging from lexical and phonological (e.g., Brice et al, 2009; Goldstein, 2005; Kohnert et al 1999; Montrul, 2006; Rao, 2019; Zentella, 1990), to grammatical and sociolinguistic (e.g., Viner, 2016, 2017, 2018a, 2018b, 2020; Erker & Otheguy, 2016; Giancaspro, 2019a, 2019b; Otheguy & Zentella, 2012). Amongst the latter, we find a highly variable syntactic feature: Spanish clitics (SC).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%