2007
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.503
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Lexical selection is not by competition: A reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm.

Abstract: The dominant view in the field of lexical access in speech production maintains that selection of a word becomes more difficult as the levels of activation of nontarget words increase--selection by competition. The authors tested this prediction in two sets of experiments. First, the authors show that participants are faster to name pictures of objects (e.g., "bed") in the context of semantically related verb distractors (e.g., sleep) compared with unrelated verb distractors (e.g., shoot). In the second set of… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

42
689
10
4

Year Published

2008
2008
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 384 publications
(747 citation statements)
references
References 111 publications
42
689
10
4
Order By: Relevance
“…This in turn fits well with models of word production that assume that lexical selection is a competitive process (e.g., Levelt et al, 1999;Piai et al, 2014;Roelofs, 1992Roelofs, , 2003. By contrast, models that do not view lexical selection as a competitive process would not predict the engagement of inhibition during lexical selection, though inhibition may be involved at a later stage shortly before articulation (e.g., Finkbeiner and Caramazza, 2006;Mahon et al, 2007).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 71%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This in turn fits well with models of word production that assume that lexical selection is a competitive process (e.g., Levelt et al, 1999;Piai et al, 2014;Roelofs, 1992Roelofs, , 2003. By contrast, models that do not view lexical selection as a competitive process would not predict the engagement of inhibition during lexical selection, though inhibition may be involved at a later stage shortly before articulation (e.g., Finkbeiner and Caramazza, 2006;Mahon et al, 2007).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 71%
“…Several models of lexical access propose that the process of lexical selection is competitive, such that the selection of a target is hindered by coactivation of competitors (e.g., Abdel Rahman and Melinger, 2009;Bloem and La Heij, 2003;Howard et al, 2006;Levelt et al, 1999;Piai et al, 2014;Roelofs, 1992Roelofs, , 2003Starreveld and La Heij, 1996). In other models, lexical selection is not seen to be a competitive process: A target is selected as soon as it has reached a threshold of activation regardless of the activation levels of other lexical items (see Finkbeiner and Caramazza, 2006;Mahon et al, 2007).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This semantic interference can be accounted for by competition between coactivated lexical entries in the same semantic context. This competition, in turn, affects selection latencies (for a review, see Levelt et al, 1999; but for an alternative view, see Finkbeiner et al, 2006, as well as Mahon et al, 2007). Co-activation of multiple lexical entries may lead to multiple potential responses active at the same time and therefore increased conflict present at the time of response.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Janssen, Carreiras, & Barber, 2011;Mahon, Costa, Peterson, Vargas, & Caramazza, 2007) which then delays naming and creates more errors. In contrast, theories of language comprehension assume that semantic interference occurs when accessing the semantic system (Forde & Humphreys, 1995Warrington & Cipolotti, 1996) due to co-activation of shared semantic features that makes distinguishing the target amongst related non-target representations more difficult.…”
Section: Neural Substrates Of Semantic Interferencementioning
confidence: 99%