1999
DOI: 10.1023/a:1022352330466
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Juror judgments in civil cases: Hindsight effects on judgments of liability for punitive damages.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
69
1

Year Published

2004
2004
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 95 publications
(72 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
1
69
1
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, it has been shown that participants' verdicts of guilt are biased toward the known verdicts of a trial when the defendant is a nonstereotypical offender (Bodenhausen, 1990) and that liability judgments are biased toward a known outcome in railroad accidents (Hastie, Schkade, & Payne, 1999) Tarasoff-type cases (LaBine & LaBine, 1996), and in cases of illegal police searches (Casper, Benedict, & Perry, 1989). Furthermore, Bryant and Brockway (1997) found that likelihood estimates of a conviction in the O. J. Simpson criminal trial made before the verdict (i.e., the acquittal) were greater than those estimates made by the same participants after the verdict was known.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, it has been shown that participants' verdicts of guilt are biased toward the known verdicts of a trial when the defendant is a nonstereotypical offender (Bodenhausen, 1990) and that liability judgments are biased toward a known outcome in railroad accidents (Hastie, Schkade, & Payne, 1999) Tarasoff-type cases (LaBine & LaBine, 1996), and in cases of illegal police searches (Casper, Benedict, & Perry, 1989). Furthermore, Bryant and Brockway (1997) found that likelihood estimates of a conviction in the O. J. Simpson criminal trial made before the verdict (i.e., the acquittal) were greater than those estimates made by the same participants after the verdict was known.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In many of these experiments, people make judgments about uncertain outcomes rather than answer questions about general knowledge. For example, the hindsight bias has been demonstrated in evaluations of sexual assault victims (Carli, 1999;Janoff-Bulman, Timko, & Carli, 1985), football games (Roese & Maniar, 1997), jury decision-making (e.g., Casper, Benedict, & Kelly, 1988;Hastie, Schkade, & Payne, 1999;Robbennolt & Sobus, 1997;Stallard & Worthington, 1998), "gustatory judgments" (Pohl, Schwarz, Sczesny, & Stahlberg, 2003), gambling behavior (Baboushkin, Hardoon, Derevensky, & Gupta, 2001), competitive team decision making (Louie, Curren, & Harich, 2000), determinations of therapist negligence (LaBine & LaBine, 1996), economic expectations (Hölzl, Kirchler, & Rodler, 2002), predictions of the likelihood of violent behavior (Cannon & Quinsey, 1995), and medical diagnoses (Arkes, Wortmann, Saville, & Harkness, 1981).…”
Section: Research On the Hindsight Biasmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Again, research in jury decision making is illustrative. Jury-eligible citizens were much more likely to find a railroad's actions negligent and an accident foreseeable in hindsight than in foresight (Hastie, Schkade, & Payne, 1999). Probability judgments of terror-related risks also appear susceptible to a hindsight bias.…”
Section: Policy Maker Responses To the Public's Probability Neglectmentioning
confidence: 99%