2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.04.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Inhibition of non-target languages in multilingual word production: Evidence from Uighur–Chinese–English trilinguals

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
22
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 32 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
4
22
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The absence of an analogous interaction for intrusion errors – which exhibited reversed dominance effects – implies a different processing locus for these dominance effects. This is consistent with our previous suggestion that reversed dominance effects reflect global application of inhibition to the dominant language (Gollan & Ferreira, 2009; see also Misra, Guo, Bobb, & Kroll, 2012; Guo, Liu, Chen, Y Li, 2013; Guo, Ma, & Liu, 2013; Van Assche, Duyck, & Gollan, 2013)—yielding consistent effects across parts of speech (and, therefore, no interaction).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…The absence of an analogous interaction for intrusion errors – which exhibited reversed dominance effects – implies a different processing locus for these dominance effects. This is consistent with our previous suggestion that reversed dominance effects reflect global application of inhibition to the dominant language (Gollan & Ferreira, 2009; see also Misra, Guo, Bobb, & Kroll, 2012; Guo, Liu, Chen, Y Li, 2013; Guo, Ma, & Liu, 2013; Van Assche, Duyck, & Gollan, 2013)—yielding consistent effects across parts of speech (and, therefore, no interaction).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…While some of these studies have also found no language preparation benefit on switch costs (Philipp et al, 2007), others have (Costa & Santesteban, 2004;Fink & Goldrick, 2015). Moreover, n-2 language repetition costs have consistently shown a language preparation benefit (Guo et al, 2013a;Philipp et al, 2007). So, in contrast with , other studies did find a language preparation effect on different costs and thus evidence for language-specific control processes.…”
Section: Multiple Loci Of Language Controlmentioning
confidence: 61%
“…A less ambiguous marker for inhibition are n-2 language repetition costs (Declerck, Thoma, et al, 2015c;Guo et al, 2013a, b;Philipp et al, 2007;Philipp & Koch, 2009). Unlike the previous markers, n-2 language repetition costs require three languages to be measured.…”
Section: N-2 Language Repetition Costs As a Marker For Inhibitionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Costa & Santesteban, 2004;Costa, Santesteban, & Ivanova, 2006;De Bruin et al, 2014;Guo, Liu, Chen, & Li, 2013;Hosoda et al, 2012;Linck et al, 2012;Philipp, Gade, & Koch, 2007;Verhoef et al, 2009). For example, Meuter and Allport (1999) required low proficiency bilinguals to switch between naming digits in L1 and L2 according to the background colour of the picture.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%