2012
DOI: 10.1080/13825585.2011.632616
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Age-related schema reliance of judgments of learning in predicting source memory

Abstract: Source memory refers to mental processes of encoding and making attributions to the origin of information. We investigated schematic effects on source attributions of younger and older adults for different schema-based types of items, and their schema-utilization of judgments of learning (JOLs) in estimating source memory. Participants studied statements presented by two speakers either as a doctor or a lawyer: those in the schema-after-encoding condition were informed their occupation only before retrieving, … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

2
15
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
2
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Konopka and Benjamin (2009) found higher JOLs for items presented by their expected source compared with their (somewhat) unexpected source. Similarly, Shi, Tang, and Liu (2012) found that participants predicted source-identification performance (i.e., the likelihood of attributing the item to the correct source) to be better for expected than unexpected source–item pairs. Both studies did, however, not assess JOSs.…”
Section: Metacognition In Source Monitoringmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…Konopka and Benjamin (2009) found higher JOLs for items presented by their expected source compared with their (somewhat) unexpected source. Similarly, Shi, Tang, and Liu (2012) found that participants predicted source-identification performance (i.e., the likelihood of attributing the item to the correct source) to be better for expected than unexpected source–item pairs. Both studies did, however, not assess JOSs.…”
Section: Metacognition In Source Monitoringmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…We designed this study to present naturalistic stimuli that would be encountered in everyday life, reasoning that participants should be able to leverage existing knowledge about the occupations, objects, and locations to optimally perform the task. Previous work has shown memory benefits when material is related to or consistent with prior knowledge (e.g., Gilboa & Marlatte, 2017; Brod, Lindenberger, Werkle-Bergner, & Shing, 2015; DeWitt, Knight, Hicks, & Ball, 2012; Anderson, 1981), and notably, this schema consistency can mitigate age-related memory deficits (Castel, 2005; Shi, Tang, & Liu, 2012). Nevertheless, deficits were quite apparent in the current study.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Older adults could be particularly sensitive to congruity in the context of social interactions. Connecting to prior work, older adults may indeed make better use of trait diagnostic information than young in some situations (Hess & Auman, 2001; Hess et al, 2005), while sometimes using schemas more than young (Mather & Johnson, 2003; Shi et al, 2012) to optimize the likelihood of positive social interactions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…While young adults rely on schemas when making decisions (Hicks & Cockman, 2003; Marsh, Cook, & Hicks, 2006), older adults may rely on schemas to a greater extent (Hess & Follett, 1994; Mather & Johnson, 2003; Shi, Tang, & Liu, 2012). Older adults also activate more stereotyped information than young to explain social situations (Radvansky, Lynchard, & von Hippel, 2009) and process incongruent behavioral information less (Hess & Tate, 1991).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%