2017
DOI: 10.1111/lang.12252
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Age Effects in First Language Attrition: Speech Perception by Korean‐English Bilinguals

Abstract: This study investigated how bilinguals’ perception of their first language (L1) differs according to age of reduced contact with L1 after immersion in a second language (L2). Twenty‐one L1 Korean‐L2 English bilinguals in the United States, ranging in age of reduced contact from 3 to 15 years, and 17 control participants in Korea were tested perceptually on three L1 contrasts differing in similarity to L2 contrasts. Compared to control participants, bilinguals were less accurate on L1‐specific contrasts, and th… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
33
1
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 50 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
2
33
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Unlike what Gürel's study revealed, a recent study investigating the role of age at onset in a group of Korean–English pre‐ and postpuberty learners showed that prepuberty learners with ages at onset up to 12 years failed to perceive L1‐specific phonemic contrasts, but they did not have problems with the contrasts that are similar to the L2 sounds (Ahn et al., ). Therefore, it appears that even phonological competence might be subject to a selective process determined by age at onset of bilingualism, but how the level of crosslinguistic similarity or competition between the L1 and L2 influences this outcome might vary based on the language pairs and the linguistic domain.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 66%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Unlike what Gürel's study revealed, a recent study investigating the role of age at onset in a group of Korean–English pre‐ and postpuberty learners showed that prepuberty learners with ages at onset up to 12 years failed to perceive L1‐specific phonemic contrasts, but they did not have problems with the contrasts that are similar to the L2 sounds (Ahn et al., ). Therefore, it appears that even phonological competence might be subject to a selective process determined by age at onset of bilingualism, but how the level of crosslinguistic similarity or competition between the L1 and L2 influences this outcome might vary based on the language pairs and the linguistic domain.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 66%
“…Echoing the above findings, a handful of studies that have attempted to fill the age‐at‐onset gap between adult heritage speakers and late bilinguals have demonstrated a discontinuity around puberty by plotting slopes for L1 proficiency by age at onset. Although participants with ages at onset over 10 to 12 years were found to be indistinguishable from monolingual controls in L1 pronunciation (Yeni‐Komshian, Flege, & Liu, ), perception of L1 speech sounds (Ahn et al., ), general proficiency (Hakuta & D'Andrea, ), verbal morphology (Silva‐Corvalán, ), and conceptualization patterns of goal‐oriented events (Bylund, ), those with ages at onset below this range were reported to show more variability, with the majority performing outside the monolingual control range.…”
Section: Background Literaturementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The latter possibility relates L1 loss and L2 gain under a unified view of plasticity in early childhood development: “the ease with which an L2 is acquired and the L1 undergoes attrition can be said to be manifestations of a generally heightened responsiveness to language exposure, which works both in acquisitional and attritional directions” ( Bylund et al, 2012 , p. 237). For a recent empirical study and review of age effects on L1 attrition, see Ahn et al (2017) .…”
Section: Dominance Plasticity Variability and Agementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Profile (Birdsong et al, 2012), the Bilingual Language Experience Calculator (Unsworth, 2013), the Language History Questionnaire (Li et al, 2014), and the custom-designed questionnaire for heritage Korean speakers used by Ahn et al (2017). Such questionnaires do not take very long to administer and provide a wealth of data about participants' language backgrounds, which are useful for determining whether study participants are representative of the target population.…”
Section: Best Practice In Treatment Of Language Backgroundmentioning
confidence: 99%