2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.10.013
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Young children, but not chimpanzees, are averse to disadvantageous and advantageous inequities

Abstract: a b s t r a c tThe age at which young children show an aversion to inequitable resource distributions, especially those favoring themselves, is unclear. It is also unclear whether great apes, as humans' nearest evolutionary relatives, have an aversion to inequitable resource distributions at all. Using a common methodology across species and child ages, the current two studies found that 3-and 4-yearold children (N = 64) not only objected when they received less than a collaborative partner but also sacrificed… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
28
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 50 publications
(32 citation statements)
references
References 54 publications
(68 reference statements)
2
28
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Though nonhuman primates possess a sensitivity to fairness, they do not do so to the same extent as humans. For example, although nonhuman primates show some signs of distributive justice, and will reject unequal distributions that favor another individual [ 36 ], they will not sacrifice their own resources to equalize a distribution, a tendency that is present in humans by age 3 [ 37 ]. Thus, the early onset of a mature set of skills related to fairness sensitivity may provide a strong foundation upon which more flexible, uniquely-human moral behaviors may develop.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Though nonhuman primates possess a sensitivity to fairness, they do not do so to the same extent as humans. For example, although nonhuman primates show some signs of distributive justice, and will reject unequal distributions that favor another individual [ 36 ], they will not sacrifice their own resources to equalize a distribution, a tendency that is present in humans by age 3 [ 37 ]. Thus, the early onset of a mature set of skills related to fairness sensitivity may provide a strong foundation upon which more flexible, uniquely-human moral behaviors may develop.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Therefore, it is perhaps not surprising that our closest living relatives do not seem to share some of our most fundamental norms of fairness and cooperation [120][121][122] (although see 123,124 ) and that there seems to be no evidence for third party punishment of norm violations harming non-kin in non-human species 125 . In contrast, third party punishment of non-kin and even strangers is frequent in humans 24,59,82 and young children already have a working knowledge of social norms 122,126,127 . The widespread prevalence of social norms may therefore well be one of the defining characteristics of our species and a crucial determinant of human cooperation.…”
Section: Summary and Open Questionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Alternatively, it can be advantageous inequity aversion, i.e., an aversion against obtaining a reward that is higher than that of a partner, which is therefore allocentric. In humans, both forms can already be observed in 3 year old toddlers (Ulber et al, 2017). Inequity aversion has also been reported in a variety of primate species (reviewed in Talbot et al, 2016;Ulber et al, 2017; but see Engelmann et al, 2017), but only in the egocentric form (but see Brosnan et al, 2010).…”
Section: Social Norms I: Universal Biologically Anchored Contentsmentioning
confidence: 93%