2011
DOI: 10.1111/j.1468-5884.2011.00488.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What can pictorial artifacts teach us about light and lightness?1

Abstract: The virtual collection of human pictorial artifacts represents a treasure for vision scientists: they are not only a showcase of possible -even if often improbable -visual experiences, but also a showcase of visual indexes and information employed by the visual system to generate our phenomenal world. In this work, we address three possible lessons that can be derived from the study of pictorial artifacts, related to the experiences of light and achromatic surface color. The first addresses the definitions of … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
12
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

3
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
0
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Although this study may appear to support the idea that an internal representation of illumination is relevant to lightness perception (i.e., the albedo hypothesis), a long discussion on whether illumination affects lightness perception must be noted (Agostini & Galmonte, 2002; Gilchrist, 2006; Schirillo, 2013; Zavagno & Daneyko, 2012; Zavagno, Daneyko, & Sakurai, 2011). Studies have argued that complex processing that collects illumination cues and subsequently constructs representations does not play a role in lightness processing (e.g., Gilchrist, 2006, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 55%
“…Although this study may appear to support the idea that an internal representation of illumination is relevant to lightness perception (i.e., the albedo hypothesis), a long discussion on whether illumination affects lightness perception must be noted (Agostini & Galmonte, 2002; Gilchrist, 2006; Schirillo, 2013; Zavagno & Daneyko, 2012; Zavagno, Daneyko, & Sakurai, 2011). Studies have argued that complex processing that collects illumination cues and subsequently constructs representations does not play a role in lightness processing (e.g., Gilchrist, 2006, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 55%
“…Distinguishing between lightness and brightness could resolve this contradiction because lightness refers to perceived reflectance, whereas brightness refers to perceived luminance or intensity of light (Schirillo, Reeves, & Arend, 1990;Zavagno, Daneyko, & Sakurai, 2011). Although Tamura et al (2016) did not seem to define lightness and brightness clearly in their experiments, probably their participants judged brightness.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…The Gestalt and the Ecological approaches share a common indifference towards the notion of veridicality, but for different reasons: if for the Ecological approach such concept is unnecessary , for the Gestalt approach it is irrelevant , given that there is no way to establish whether a percept is veridical with respect to the distal source of stimulation, as knowledge of such source comes from the percept itself, or is mediated by the use of some instrumentation, which however cannot tell what is more veridical. Moreover physical properties of a distal stimulus are only rough correlates of what is perceived (Zavagno et al, 2011a , b ). For instance, the luminance of an achromatic surface is the physical correlate of lightness (achromatic surface color), which however, does not correlate with the luminance of the physical surface: a sheet of paper with reflectance 70% may appear light gray even if its luminance changes dramatically, because lightness is a contextual experience, functional to the characteristics of the scene in relation to the perceiver.…”
Section: Theoretical Frameworkmentioning
confidence: 99%