2007
DOI: 10.2307/30035543
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Vocabulary Intervention for Kindergarten Students: Comparing Extended Instruction to Embedded Instruction and Incidental Exposure

Abstract: The purpose of the two studies reported in this article was to evaluate the effectiveness of extended vocabulary instruction during storybook reading with kindergarten students within a small-group intervention setting. Extended vocabulary instruction is characterized by explicit teaching that includes both contextual and definitional information, multiple exposures to target words in varied contexts, and experiences that promote deep processing of word meanings. In Study One, we compared extended instruction … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

11
213
1
25

Year Published

2010
2010
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 229 publications
(250 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
11
213
1
25
Order By: Relevance
“…At pretest, children averaged 14 words correctly identified and at posttest, children averaged 21 words correctly identified, which corresponds to learning of about seven words overall. Similar results have been reported in a number of studies (e.g., Beck et al, 1982;Biemiller & Boote, 2006;Coyne et al, 2007;Penno, Wilkinson, & Moore, 2002), suggesting that we have made little headway since 1982 when Beck and co-authors noted that students' learning of words targeted in their intervention was far less than what they expected: "Acquiring word meanings to a high level is not an easy task, even with intensive instruction" (p. 518). Unfortunately in this study our scoring metric didn't allow us to determine the exact number of words children learned in each condition.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 64%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…At pretest, children averaged 14 words correctly identified and at posttest, children averaged 21 words correctly identified, which corresponds to learning of about seven words overall. Similar results have been reported in a number of studies (e.g., Beck et al, 1982;Biemiller & Boote, 2006;Coyne et al, 2007;Penno, Wilkinson, & Moore, 2002), suggesting that we have made little headway since 1982 when Beck and co-authors noted that students' learning of words targeted in their intervention was far less than what they expected: "Acquiring word meanings to a high level is not an easy task, even with intensive instruction" (p. 518). Unfortunately in this study our scoring metric didn't allow us to determine the exact number of words children learned in each condition.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 64%
“…Although there is sufficient information in the literature for professionals to develop vocabulary interventions that are explicit and robust, does increasing the dosage of these interventions help those children who most need vocabulary enhancement? Although this study shows a general benefit of double-dose instruction over single-dose instruction, and complements prior work attempting to do the same (e.g., Coyne et al, 2007), will this approach help the most vulnerable children across the grades? Furthermore, it is important to determine the longer term impacts of robust vocabulary interventions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 73%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Des études ont révélé que dans les milieux défavorisés, les jeunes enfants vivent moins d'expériences soutenant le développement des compétences orales et écrites que dans les milieux plus aisés (Coyne, McCoach et Knapp, 2007;Hart et Risley, 2003;Korat, 2005). Apthorp (2006) estime d'ailleurs que des élèves ayant bénéficié de moins de stimulations langagières peuvent, dès la maternelle, connaitre près de 4 000 mots de moins que leurs pairs bénéficiant d'expériences riches et variées à la maison.…”
Section: Problématiqueunclassified
“…This also explains the relatively low scores on the L1 meaning-matching test among the low-proficiency children in the present study. In other words, knowledge of a second language influences the ability to draw on L1 or other cognitive resources and affects the rate of efficiency of processing higher-level cues, such as the semantic, syntactic, and contextual cues available when teachers read aloud (Coyne, McCoach, & Kapp, 2007). Such a causal relation is apparently bidirectional (Robbins & Ehri, 1994).…”
Section: *P < 05mentioning
confidence: 99%