2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.08.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The structure and stability of temperament from infancy to toddlerhood: A one-year prospective study

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

5
37
0
16

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 64 publications
(58 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
5
37
0
16
Order By: Relevance
“…Moreover, whilst Effortful Control is conventionally only measured as a dimension of temperament from 18 months (see section on Measuring Effortful control), there is a strong relationship between scores on this factor, and parent report of infant regulatory capacity, as indexed by the Regulatory Capacity/Orienting factor (Putnam, Rothbart, & Gartstein, 2008). For example, composite scores from the Cuddliness and Duration of Orienting scales at 8-13 months are associated with levels of Effortful Control at 20-25 months of age (Casalin, Luyten, Vliegen,& Meurs, 2012).…”
Section: Early Individual Differences In Reactivity and Self-regulatimentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Moreover, whilst Effortful Control is conventionally only measured as a dimension of temperament from 18 months (see section on Measuring Effortful control), there is a strong relationship between scores on this factor, and parent report of infant regulatory capacity, as indexed by the Regulatory Capacity/Orienting factor (Putnam, Rothbart, & Gartstein, 2008). For example, composite scores from the Cuddliness and Duration of Orienting scales at 8-13 months are associated with levels of Effortful Control at 20-25 months of age (Casalin, Luyten, Vliegen,& Meurs, 2012).…”
Section: Early Individual Differences In Reactivity and Self-regulatimentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, it is argued that highly reactive infants who become extremely distressed in response to stimulation become so disrupted they are unable to develop the internal mechanisms necessary for selfregulation (Fox & Calkins, 1993); so much so that children's negative reactivity has been identified as a risk factor for poor-regulation (Allhusen et al, 2004;Raikes, Robinson, Bradley, Raikes, & Ayoub, 2007;Rothbart & Bates, 1998). Inversely, positive affectivity may be a protective factor; infant scores on the High-Intensity Pleasure, Smiling and Laughter and Positive Anticipation scales of the IBQ have all been shown to predict toddler and/or preschool Effortful Control scores (Casalin et al, 2012;Komsi et al, 2006;Putnam et al, 2008).…”
Section: Early Individual Differences In Reactivity and Self-regulatimentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Several studies have provided evidence for stability of child temperament over time using parent reports (e.g., Bould, Joinson, Sterne, & Araya, 2013;Casalin, Luyten, Vliegen, & Meurs, 2012;Guerin & Gottfried, 1994) and observational methods (e.g., Dyson, Olino, Durbin, Goldsmith, Bufferd, Miller et al, 2015). However, there has not yet been a study that examined the structural stability of child temperament over time using measurement invariance testing in the context of exploratory structural equation modeling.…”
Section: Strengths and Weaknesses Of The Three Studies And Future Dirmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Contemporary research generally characterizes temperament as stylistic and relatively stable traits that subsume intrinsic (i.e., biological) tendencies to act and react in somewhat predictable ways to people and events (Casalin, Luyten, Vliegens, & Meurs, 2012;Rothbart & Hwang, 2002;Worobey, 2001). Although the terms temperament and personality at times are used synonymously, differences exist in their origins, emergence, and robustness.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%