2003
DOI: 10.1080/02724980244000701
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Macrostructure of Informal Arguments: A Proposed Model and Analysis

Abstract: Theories of informal reasoning and critical thinking often maintain that everyday, informal arguments can be classified into types based on the specific organization that the premises or reasons enter into in their support for the conclusion (Snoeck Henkemans, 2000; Vorobej, 1995b). Three general types are identified: convergent, coordinately linked, and subordinately linked arguments. There has been no empirical research, however, to determine whether these structural distinctions have any psychological reali… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
26
1

Year Published

2007
2007
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
0
26
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The little research that does exist in this area is mixed. Some studies (e.g., Ricco 2003, cited by M&S) suggest that for simple arguments, adults can, when prompted, differentiate between linked and convergent arguments. Other studies, however, suggest that, even for simple arguments, untrained college students can identify the conclusion but without prompting are poor at both identifying the premises and how the premises support the conclusion (Harrell 2006;2011).…”
Section: Conclusion L Inferencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The little research that does exist in this area is mixed. Some studies (e.g., Ricco 2003, cited by M&S) suggest that for simple arguments, adults can, when prompted, differentiate between linked and convergent arguments. Other studies, however, suggest that, even for simple arguments, untrained college students can identify the conclusion but without prompting are poor at both identifying the premises and how the premises support the conclusion (Harrell 2006;2011).…”
Section: Conclusion L Inferencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…They can follow the commitments of the different speakers and determine, at any given point of the argument, who has the burden of proof (Bailenson & Rips, 1996;Rips, 1998). They understand the macrostructure of arguments (Ricco, 2003). They are often able to spot the classical fallacies of argumentation, such as ad hominem, petitio principii (begging the question), or circular reasoning (Neuman, Glassner, & Weinstock, 2004;Neuman, Weinstock, & Glasner, 2006;Rips, 2002;Weinstock, Neuman, & Tabak, 2004).…”
Section: Are People Good At Arguing?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This assessment is made explicit through criteria evaluating a) the "use of pre-defined argument schemes", such as those proposed by Walton (1996) or the argument structures used by Ricco (2003) or Neuman (2003); b) the "use of correct and valid evidence" and/or the avoidance of pseudoevidence (Kuhn, 1991); and c) the "explicit relations" among argument elements/premises, either through diagramming arguments (Lund, Molinari, Séjourné, & Baker, 2007), or as a verbal justification of the relation between claims and evidence (Sandoval & Millwood, 2008;McNeill, 2008).…”
Section: Argument Assessment Criteriamentioning
confidence: 99%