1990
DOI: 10.1016/0749-5978(90)90032-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effect of choosing on confidence in choice

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
99
1

Year Published

1991
1991
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 200 publications
(107 citation statements)
references
References 12 publications
7
99
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Several researchers have proposed that confidence in an answer is influenced more strongly by evidence that speaks for it than by evidence that speaks against it (Arkes, Christensen, Lai, & Blumer, 1987;McKenzie, 1997;Sniezek, Paese, & Switzer, 1990). Thus, Koriat et al (1980) proposed that whereas the choice of an answer is based on the balance of evidence for and against each of the alternatives (Griffin & Tversky, 1992;McKenzie, 1997McKenzie, , 1998, confidence in the answer chosen is based on the attempt to justify that answer, which results in overweighting of the evidence that supports it.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Several researchers have proposed that confidence in an answer is influenced more strongly by evidence that speaks for it than by evidence that speaks against it (Arkes, Christensen, Lai, & Blumer, 1987;McKenzie, 1997;Sniezek, Paese, & Switzer, 1990). Thus, Koriat et al (1980) proposed that whereas the choice of an answer is based on the balance of evidence for and against each of the alternatives (Griffin & Tversky, 1992;McKenzie, 1997McKenzie, , 1998, confidence in the answer chosen is based on the attempt to justify that answer, which results in overweighting of the evidence that supports it.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Of course, this should also be the case if the bias in favor of one of the answers has already developed during the search for pertinent considerations (Sniezek et al, 1990;Tsai, Klayman, & Hastie, in press).…”
Section: University Of Haifa Haifa Israelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, one can consider the overconfidence phenomenon in almanac question studies as a form of base rate neglect. Despite the fact that people know the fairly low relative frequency with which they are choosing the correct alternative (Gigerenzer, 1991;Sniezek et al, 1990), they continue to express high confidence that they have chosen the correct alternative on an item-byitem basis. The information they possess about the base rate with which they answer such questions correctly (i.e., their prior probability of answering a question correctly) seems to have little impact on their likelihood judgments in response to particular items.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As discussed presently, there are many other interesting aspects of external correspondence that may or may not be affected by a relative-frequency elicitation question. A final reason to study the effects of a relative-frequency question on external correspondence is research showing that people performing a general-knowledge task often estimate the number or percentage of general-knowledge questions they answered correctly during the experiment with a high degree of accuracy (Gigerenzer, Hoffrage, & Kleinbölting, 1991;Sniezek, Paese, & Switzer, 1990). This suggests that people may have access to accurate relative-frequency information to which a relativefrequency elicitation question may draw their attention.…”
Section: External Correspondencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Further, accountability seems to decrease levels of overconfidence, such that overconfidence decreases if choice answers are observed (Sniezek et al, 1990). All else equal, the relative simplicity of the experiment, and the fact that subjects report choice answers to the experimenter (e.g.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%