2011
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0028675
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Temporal Integration of Movement: The Time-Course of Motion Streaks Revealed by Masking

Abstract: Temporal integration in the visual system causes fast-moving objects to leave oriented ‘motion streaks’ in their wake, which could be used to facilitate motion direction perception. Temporal integration is thought to occur over 100 ms in early cortex, although this has never been tested for motion streaks. Here we compare the ability of fast-moving (‘streaky’) and slow-moving fields of dots to mask briefly flashed gratings either parallel or orthogonal to the motion trajectory. Gratings were presented at vario… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
12
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
2
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, rapid retinal motion produces responses both in motion-selective cells tuned to that direction, and in orientation-selective cells tuned to an orientation aligned with the axis of the motion—the motion-streak. Psychophysical evidence from orientation detection and after-effects, as well as recent neuroimaging data, is consistent with the view that motion-streaks excite orientation-tuned cells in the human visual system (Alais et al, 2011; Apthorp et al, 2011, 2013). Geisler (1999) proposed that the outputs of motion- and orientation-selective cells are combined in visual cortex to create a “spatial motion-direction” (SMD) sensor tuned to both streak orientation and motion direction.…”
Section: Motion-streakssupporting
confidence: 81%
“…Thus, rapid retinal motion produces responses both in motion-selective cells tuned to that direction, and in orientation-selective cells tuned to an orientation aligned with the axis of the motion—the motion-streak. Psychophysical evidence from orientation detection and after-effects, as well as recent neuroimaging data, is consistent with the view that motion-streaks excite orientation-tuned cells in the human visual system (Alais et al, 2011; Apthorp et al, 2011, 2013). Geisler (1999) proposed that the outputs of motion- and orientation-selective cells are combined in visual cortex to create a “spatial motion-direction” (SMD) sensor tuned to both streak orientation and motion direction.…”
Section: Motion-streakssupporting
confidence: 81%
“…On the other hand, Cesaro and Agostini examined motionpath distortion for speeds of 0.78, 1.55, and 38/s with a single dot of a diameter of 0.0228. While the speeds adopted by Cesaro and Agostini are sufficient to generate a motion streak, it can be questioned whether a motion streak is perceptible given the very small size of the stimulus, and the fact that motion streaks are likely to be low contrast given the fact that they arise from temporal integration (Alais, Apthorp, Karmann, & Cass, 2011;Apthorp, Cass, & Alais, 2010;Kelly, 1961). The fact that increasing speed produces a reduction in the ''slalom'' effect is consistent with a reduction in the visibility of the object at faster speeds.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…The RDKs consisted of 100% peak contrast dots with a Laplacian of Gaussian (LoG) profile (Curran et al 2009) with ϭ 0.05°and a peak spatial frequency of 10.6 cycles/deg. The spatial profile of the isotropic LoG dots minimized the artifactual orientation signals known as "motion streaks" resulting from the temporal integration of dot trajectories (Alais et al 2011;Geisler 1999), which may have arisen with nonfiltered dot stimuli. The RDKs had a dot density of 5.9 dots/deg 2 and moved at an average speed of 1.78°/s.…”
Section: Visual Stimulimentioning
confidence: 99%