2008
DOI: 10.1007/s00426-008-0162-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Strategic capacity sharing between two tasks: evidence from tasks with the same and with different task sets

Abstract: The goal of the present study was to investigate the costs and benefits of different degrees of strategic parallel processing between two tasks. In a series of experiments with the dual-task flanker paradigm, participants were either instructed to process the tasks serially or in parallel, or--in a control condition--they received no specific instruction. Results showed that the participants were able to adjust the degree of parallel processing as instructed in a flexible manner. Parallel processing of the two… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

4
68
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 68 publications
(72 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
4
68
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Kahneman, 1973;Kondo et al, 2004;Logan & Gordon, 2001;Meyer & Kieras, 1997a, 1997bNavon & Miller, 2002;Tombu & Jolicoeur, 2003). The assumption that dual-task performance is not constrained by a structural responseselection bottleneck is supported by several findings (e.g., Fan et al, 2012;Hübner & Lehle, 2007;Israel & Cohen, 2011;Kahneman, 1973;Karlin & Kerstenbaum, 1968;Lehle & Hübner, 2009;Leonhard & Ulrich, 2011;Logan & Gordon, 2001;Meyer & Kieras, 1997a, 1997bNavon & Miller, 2002;Pannebakker et al, 2011;Schvaneveldt, 1969;Szameitat et al, 2002Szameitat et al, , 2006Tombu & Jolicoeur, 2003). For example, there is evidence that the magnitude of dual-task interference can be eliminated with practice (e.g., Schumacher et al, 1999Schumacher et al, , 2001, but see Ruthruff et al, 2006;Van Selst et al, 1999) or by increasing the number of short SOAs in an experimental block of trials (Miller et al, 2009).…”
Section: Passive Queuing and Active Scheduling Accountsmentioning
confidence: 94%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Kahneman, 1973;Kondo et al, 2004;Logan & Gordon, 2001;Meyer & Kieras, 1997a, 1997bNavon & Miller, 2002;Tombu & Jolicoeur, 2003). The assumption that dual-task performance is not constrained by a structural responseselection bottleneck is supported by several findings (e.g., Fan et al, 2012;Hübner & Lehle, 2007;Israel & Cohen, 2011;Kahneman, 1973;Karlin & Kerstenbaum, 1968;Lehle & Hübner, 2009;Leonhard & Ulrich, 2011;Logan & Gordon, 2001;Meyer & Kieras, 1997a, 1997bNavon & Miller, 2002;Pannebakker et al, 2011;Schvaneveldt, 1969;Szameitat et al, 2002Szameitat et al, , 2006Tombu & Jolicoeur, 2003). For example, there is evidence that the magnitude of dual-task interference can be eliminated with practice (e.g., Schumacher et al, 1999Schumacher et al, , 2001, but see Ruthruff et al, 2006;Van Selst et al, 1999) or by increasing the number of short SOAs in an experimental block of trials (Miller et al, 2009).…”
Section: Passive Queuing and Active Scheduling Accountsmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Meyer et al, 2003) at short compared to long SOAs. Alternatively, these RT1 effects may occur when central capacity is shared between the Task 1 and Task 2 processes (e.g., Kahneman, 1973;Lehle & Hübner, 2009;Navon & Miller, 2002;Tombu & Jolicoeur, 2003).…”
Section: Active Scheduling Versus Central Capacity Sharingmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, the effect of practice on the magnitude of the dual-task interference is also different across studies (e.g., Karlin & Kestenbaum, 1968;Ruthruff, Johnston, Van Selst, Whitsell, & Remington, 2003;Schumacher et al, 2001;Van Selst & Jolicoeur, 1997;Van Selst, Ruthruff, & Johnston, 1999). These and other findings challenge the assumption of a structural response-selection bottleneck in dualtask performance (e.g., Hübner & Lehle, 2007;Israel & Cohen, 2011;Karlin & Kestenbaum, 1968;Lehle & Hübner, 2009;Leonhard & Ulrich, 2011;Meyer & Kieras, 1997;Miller et al, 2009;Navon & Miller, 2002;Pannebakker et al, 2011;Schumacher et al, 1999Schumacher et al, , 2001Schvaneveldt, 1969;Szameitat, Schubert, Müller, & Von Cramon, 2002;Szameitat, Lepsien, von Cramon, Sterr, & Schubert, 2006;Tombu & Jolicoeur, 2003). Although under the strategic bottleneck account (Meyer & Kieras, 1997;Piai et al, 2011;Roelofs, 2007Roelofs, , 2008a, a responseselection bottleneck is optional rather than obligatory (i.e., response selection in Tasks 1 and 2 may, in principle, occur in parallel), the present findings suggest that participants seem to have a very strong preference for not overlapping responseselection processes in dual-task performance.…”
Section: The Nature Of the Processing Bottleneckmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…We also know that multitasking costs are modulated by different factors, such as for instance, by the time offered for the tasks to be prepared (Altmann, 2004;Hoffmann, Kiesel, & Sebald, 2003;Koch, 2001;Meiran, 1996;Poljac, de Haan, van Galen, 2006) or by task order (Hirsch, Nolden, & Koch, 2017;Luria & Meiran, 2003;Szameitat, Lepsien, von Cramon, Sterr, & Schubert, 2006;Töllner, Strobach, Schubert, & Müller, 2012). In addition to these examples of task-related factors, the field has informed us that also individual differences play a critical role, such as age (Cepeda, Kramer, & Gonzalez de Sather, 2001;Kray & Ferdinand, 2014;Maquestiaux, Hartley, & Bertsch, 2004;Reimers & Maylor, 2005) or one's own individual multitasking style (Lehle & Huebner, 2009;Miller, Ulrich, & Rolke, 2009;Reissland & Manzey, 2016).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%