2011
DOI: 10.1037/a0021923
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Speaking rate from proximal and distal contexts is used during word segmentation.

Abstract: A series of eye-tracking and categorization experiments investigated the use of speaking-rate information in the segmentation of Dutch ambiguous-word sequences. Juncture phonemes with ambiguous durations (e.g., [s] in 'eens (s)peer,' "once (s)pear," [t] in 'nooit (t)rap,' "never staircase/quick") were perceived as longer and hence more often as word-initial when following a fast than a slow context sentence. Listeners used speaking-rate information as soon as it became available. Rate information from a cont… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

9
56
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 49 publications
(65 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
(100 reference statements)
9
56
0
Order By: Relevance
“…So listeners do not interpret an incoming sound solely on the basis of its acoustic properties, but take speaker-specific properties (like the available F1 range) into account. Another example of how listeners tune in to a particular talker is provided by Reinisch, Jesse, and McQueen (2011). They showed that the perception of a juncture phoneme with a constant, but ambiguous duration (e.g., [s] in Dutch eens (s)peer "once (s)pear" changed depending on the rate at which the preceding context was spoken.…”
Section: Speaker-specific Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…So listeners do not interpret an incoming sound solely on the basis of its acoustic properties, but take speaker-specific properties (like the available F1 range) into account. Another example of how listeners tune in to a particular talker is provided by Reinisch, Jesse, and McQueen (2011). They showed that the perception of a juncture phoneme with a constant, but ambiguous duration (e.g., [s] in Dutch eens (s)peer "once (s)pear" changed depending on the rate at which the preceding context was spoken.…”
Section: Speaker-specific Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recently, several lines of research have demonstrated that nonlocal information, specifically the signal-based factors of distal (i.e., nonadjacent context) speech rate and rhythm, can influence spoken word segmentation and recognition (e.g., Baese-Berk et al, 2014;Dilley & Pitt, 2010;Holt, 2005;Reinisch, Jesse, & McQueen, 2011). For example, the perception of entire syllables or words can be affected by distal speech rate.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Through this process, they deal with the large variability in speaking rate during speech perception. That is, following a fast context sentence, a given sound is interpreted as longer than when it follows a slow context sentence (e.g., Ainsworth, 1972Ainsworth, , 1974Allen & Miller, 2001;Dilley & Pitt, 2010;Kidd, 1989;Miller, 1981Miller, , 1987Miller & Dexter, 1988;Newman & Sawusch, 2009;Reinisch, Jesse, & McQueen, 2011;Reinisch & Sjerps, 2013;Summerfield, 1981). For example, the wordinitial stop voicing contrast in English (e.g., /g/ vs. /k/) is mainly cued by temporal properties, namely duration of voice onset time (VOT).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As a result, context information influences whether listeners hear words, for instance, as goat or coat. This information can aid speech perception, especially in the case of ambiguous phonemes (e.g., Newman & Sawusch, 2009;Reinisch et al 2011;Sawusch & Newman, 2000) and even when other potential cues are available to the listener (Reinisch & Sjerps, 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation