1989
DOI: 10.1080/00224545.1989.9713779
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Some Effects of Sex-Linked Clothing and Gender Schema on the Stereotyping of Infants

Abstract: Two predictions were evaluated: first, that given minimal information about an infant, individuals would use sex-related cues (i.e., clothing) to categorize, evaluate, and make attributions about the infant, and second, that gender schematic individuals would be more likely than gender aschematic individuals to use such sex-related cues. On the basis of the Bem Sex Role Inventory, American college students were classified as either gender schematic (masculine or feminine) or gender aschematic (undifferentiated… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

1992
1992
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
(22 reference statements)
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The environmental cues to gender that signal masculine or feminine stereotypical constructs are quite diverse: Vocal quality [ 43 , 44 ], facial structure [ 45 ], facial coloring [ 46 ], handwriting [ 47 ], hairstyle [ 48 ], clothing [ 49 , 50 ], and the color of clothing or other objects associated with individuals [ 51 ]. The concept of masculinity or femininity that is marketed with a perfume seems able to trigger cross-modal odor-color gender stereotypes as well; a series of studies have demonstrated that when people smell a fine fragrance, a specific color comes to mind that differs based on whether the perfume is marketed to men (blues or greens) or women (pinks or yellow) [ 52 ].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The environmental cues to gender that signal masculine or feminine stereotypical constructs are quite diverse: Vocal quality [ 43 , 44 ], facial structure [ 45 ], facial coloring [ 46 ], handwriting [ 47 ], hairstyle [ 48 ], clothing [ 49 , 50 ], and the color of clothing or other objects associated with individuals [ 51 ]. The concept of masculinity or femininity that is marketed with a perfume seems able to trigger cross-modal odor-color gender stereotypes as well; a series of studies have demonstrated that when people smell a fine fragrance, a specific color comes to mind that differs based on whether the perfume is marketed to men (blues or greens) or women (pinks or yellow) [ 52 ].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The Journal of Genetic Psychologv unidentified infant as male or female (Hildebrandt & Fitzgerald, 1979;Leone & Robertson , 1989;Seavey, Katz, & Zalk , 1975). A fair summary of thi s literature is Bims 's ( 1976) conclusion that there are almost no consistent differences in the behavior of male and female infants but that adults' perceptions of infants and behaviors toward infants may reflect to varying degrees the existence of sex role stereotypes.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…irl (Poludi & Gullo, 1986); or with a photograph (Bell, 1981 ;Hildebrandt & Fitzgerald, 1977 , audiotape (Bell, 1981 ;Rothbart & Maccoby, 1966) , or videotape (Condry & Condry, 1976;Gurwitz & Dodge , 1975 ;Haugh , Hoffman , & Cowan, 1980;Holman & Williamson , 1979;Leone & Robertson , 1989;Meyer & Sobieszek, 1972) of an infant labeled as male for some subjects and as female for others. With this method the stimulus is the same for all subjects (a lthough this is not completely true in all studies ; e. g., Zucker & Corter, 1980); therefore, any consistent differences in observers ' evaluations of the infant(s) as a function of labeling can be interpreted to show that their views about sex roles are stereotyped .…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations