2011
DOI: 10.1152/jn.00394.2010
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Sensorimotor adaptation error signals are derived from realistic predictions of movement outcomes

Abstract: Neural systems that control movement maintain accuracy by adaptively altering motor commands in response to errors. It is often assumed that the error signal that drives adaptation is equivalent to the sensory error observed at the conclusion of a movement; for saccades, this is typically the visual (retinal) error. However, we instead propose that the adaptation error signal is derived as the difference between the observed visual error and a realistic prediction of movement outcome. Using a modified saccade-… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
77
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 76 publications
(85 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
(65 reference statements)
5
77
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In the Large-On condition, retinal error was 0.69 Ϯ 0.50°and target step size was Ϫ1.66 Ϯ 0.31°. Again, granting that small positive retinal errors cause amplitudedecreasing adaptation (Wong and Shelhamer 2010), if both errors were equally effective at evoking adaptation there should be ϳ40% more adaptation in session 2 relative to session 1; instead, we found 200% more adaptation in session 2. Specifically, the amplitude decrease was 0.31°in session 1 (from 11.82 Ϯ 0.12°to 11.51 Ϯ 0.15°) and 1.54°in session 2 (from 11.65 Ϯ 0.12°to 10.11 Ϯ 0.12°).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 48%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In the Large-On condition, retinal error was 0.69 Ϯ 0.50°and target step size was Ϫ1.66 Ϯ 0.31°. Again, granting that small positive retinal errors cause amplitudedecreasing adaptation (Wong and Shelhamer 2010), if both errors were equally effective at evoking adaptation there should be ϳ40% more adaptation in session 2 relative to session 1; instead, we found 200% more adaptation in session 2. Specifically, the amplitude decrease was 0.31°in session 1 (from 11.82 Ϯ 0.12°to 11.51 Ϯ 0.15°) and 1.54°in session 2 (from 11.65 Ϯ 0.12°to 10.11 Ϯ 0.12°).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 48%
“…Here we altered the distributions of retinal error by removing the target on selected trials. Wong and Shelhamer (2010) took advantage of the fact that large saccades tend to fall short of the target and displaced the target during the saccade to a position about midway between its initial position and the mean saccade landing position. This situation created a mean positive retinal error, which would be expected to cause an increase in saccade amplitude.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We have previously suggested that two independent error signals, 1) the discrepancy between the desired and actual movement, known as the sensorimotor error signal (Wong and Shelhamer 2011), and 2) the discrepancy between visual and proprioceptive estimates of hand position, which we refer to as the cross-sensory error signal, may be primarily responsible for changes in movements and felt hand position, respectively . Furthermore, changes in felt hand position may contribute to reach adaptation when only a cross-sensory error signal is present Salomonczyk et al 2013).…”
Section: Motor Adaptation Vs Sensory Recalibrationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is increasing evidence that saccade adaptation uses sensory prediction errors as its key error signal (Bahcall and Kowler 2000;Chen-Harris et al 2008;Ethier et al 2008;Wong and Shelhamer 2011;Collins and Wallman 2012;Herman et al 2013b), rather than simple sensory (retinal) error (Wallman and Fuchs 1998;Noto and Robinson 2001). In the parlance of internal model theories, a "forward model" estimates the predicted sensory error of a movement, while an "inverse model" translates the desired movement goal into the necessary motor commands to control the dynamics of the eye and muscle plant.…”
Section: Bayesian Switching and Integration Of Sensory Predictionsmentioning
confidence: 99%