2012
DOI: 10.1152/jn.00746.2011
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The relative importance of retinal error and prediction in saccadic adaptation

Abstract: Collins T, Wallman J. The relative importance of retinal error and prediction in saccadic adaptation.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

5
63
2

Year Published

2012
2012
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 62 publications
(72 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
5
63
2
Order By: Relevance
“…In agreement with previous reports (Cherici et al, 2012), noticeable idiosyncratic variability existed in the amplitude distribution of fixational saccades, so that the mean amplitude varied across observers. As a consequence, because the intrasaccadic target step was proportional to the saccade amplitude, subjects with larger fixational saccades were also exposed, on average, to larger target displacements.…”
Section: Adaptation Of Fixational Saccadessupporting
confidence: 92%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In agreement with previous reports (Cherici et al, 2012), noticeable idiosyncratic variability existed in the amplitude distribution of fixational saccades, so that the mean amplitude varied across observers. As a consequence, because the intrasaccadic target step was proportional to the saccade amplitude, subjects with larger fixational saccades were also exposed, on average, to larger target displacements.…”
Section: Adaptation Of Fixational Saccadessupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Saccadic adaptation is known to rely on an extraretinal signal, an internal copy of the motor command known as corollary discharge or efference copy, to predict the retinal position of the target after the saccade (Bahcall and Kowler, 2000;Wong and Shelhamer, 2011;Collins and Wallman, 2012;. A large body of evidence indicates that such corollary discharge signals are also important for establishing stable spatial representations across saccades (Ross et al, 2001;Sommer and Wurtz, 2002;Hamker et al, 2008;Cavanagh et al, 2010;Hafed, 2013;Poletti et al, 2013a), perhaps even spatiotopic ones, as has been recently shown for large saccade adaptation (Zimmermann et al, 2011). In addition to creating the need for motor tuning, the stimulus manipulations present during saccadic adaptation experiments also affect perceptual representations, as they cause systematic mismatches between the postsaccadic retinal inputs and their presaccadic motor predictions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…saccades: Walker & McSorley, 2006Burke & Barnes, 2008a;Collins & Wallman, 2012). This view has strong resonance with results from recent neuroimaging experiments where Silk et al (2010) have suggested that neural activity in the right SMG reflects the generation of spatial map or co-ordinate system via which information about target/object location can be retained.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…There is increasing evidence that saccade adaptation uses sensory prediction errors as its key error signal (Bahcall and Kowler 2000;Chen-Harris et al 2008;Ethier et al 2008;Wong and Shelhamer 2011;Collins and Wallman 2012;Herman et al 2013b), rather than simple sensory (retinal) error (Wallman and Fuchs 1998;Noto and Robinson 2001). In the parlance of internal model theories, a "forward model" estimates the predicted sensory error of a movement, while an "inverse model" translates the desired movement goal into the necessary motor commands to control the dynamics of the eye and muscle plant.…”
Section: Bayesian Switching and Integration Of Sensory Predictionsmentioning
confidence: 99%