Seeing in Depth 2008
DOI: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195367607.003.0015
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Seeing Motion in depth

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

3
104
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 54 publications
(107 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
3
104
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The effect is interpreted as reflecting the combination of disparity information with familiarity and other cues to depth (Hartung et al 2005), or the operation of Emmert's law (Kroliczak et al, in press). If it results from misperceived distance of the illusion, the flattening may also reflect the fact that size scales with 1ad whereas disparity scales as 1ad 2 , a fact well-known to cause illusory flattening (for review, see Howard and Rogers 2002). The effect of the relative depth of the illusion is considered in experiment 6.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The effect is interpreted as reflecting the combination of disparity information with familiarity and other cues to depth (Hartung et al 2005), or the operation of Emmert's law (Kroliczak et al, in press). If it results from misperceived distance of the illusion, the flattening may also reflect the fact that size scales with 1ad whereas disparity scales as 1ad 2 , a fact well-known to cause illusory flattening (for review, see Howard and Rogers 2002). The effect of the relative depth of the illusion is considered in experiment 6.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…are not available. A directional light source from left-above of the scene is assumed in accordance with the human visual system's assumptions ( [21], section 24.4.2).…”
Section: Preprocessingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The positional differences of an object's projections on an observer's left and right retinas, or their binocular disparity, is a strong cue for perceiving sufficiently near depths 1,2 . For distant objects, monocular cues, such as T-junctions, may be used to determine relative depth when a nearer object occludes part of a farther object, and the visible parts of an occluded object are often perceptually linked together behind the occluder (Figures 1b,c,e,f) 2 . How does the visual cortex generate such percepts, and what are the perceptual units that are used to do so?…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Most previous stereopsis models [3][4][5][6][7][8][9][10] analysed how left and right eye contours are matched a f e b c d (the correspondence problem) 2,3 , but not how matching leads to 3D perceptual groupings that support conscious percepts of surface lightness and color. Here, three types of 3D surface percepts that can be generated by RDS are explained and simulated in order to illuminate crucial brain processes of 3D visual perception.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation