2016
DOI: 10.1002/acp.3274
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Public Attitudes on the Ethics of Deceptively Planting False Memories to Motivate Healthy Behavior

Abstract: SummaryResearchers have proposed that planting false memories could have positive behavioral consequences. The idea of deceptively planting ‘beneficial’ false memories outside of the laboratory raises important ethical questions, but how might the general public appraise this moral dilemma? In two studies, participants from the USA and UK read about a fictional ‘false‐memory therapy’ that led people to adopt healthy behaviors. Participants then reported their attitudes toward the acceptability of this therapy,… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Another possible explanation for subjects' reluctance to erase their negative memories is that they were wary of the general consequences of erasing a memory. For example, we know that many people believe that changing people's memories in therapy might have unintended negative consequences, and it is possible that some subjects decided not to erase their memory for this reason (Nash, Berkowitz, & Roche, 2016). But this explanation cannot fully account for our results.…”
Section: Alternate Explanationsmentioning
confidence: 80%
“…Another possible explanation for subjects' reluctance to erase their negative memories is that they were wary of the general consequences of erasing a memory. For example, we know that many people believe that changing people's memories in therapy might have unintended negative consequences, and it is possible that some subjects decided not to erase their memory for this reason (Nash, Berkowitz, & Roche, 2016). But this explanation cannot fully account for our results.…”
Section: Alternate Explanationsmentioning
confidence: 80%
“…Although M&C do not discuss nonbelieved memories, they do give greater attention to the adaptiveness of false memories. Susceptibility to false memories might offer numerous specific benefits to the rememberer (Bernstein & Loftus 2009;Howe 2011;Nash et al 2016), but M&C propose that this susceptibility is also generally adaptive, because convincing ourselves of selfserving beliefs is an essential first step toward convincing others. They further propose a reciprocal benefit: adopting other people's beliefs into our own recollections can be "communicatively useful" as a means of enhancing our epistemic authority.…”
Section: Jennifer Nagelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In our study, beliefs about the therapist’s competency depended on outcome information surrounding the false memory. Indeed, recent studies suggest that the public’s regard for techniques that elicit false memories is mixed, even in instances where the false memories may have beneficial effects (Nash, Berkowitz, & Roche, 2016). In the present study, although foresight participants did not relate competency to suggestiveness, hindsight participants did, and they rated the competency of the therapist as significantly lower than did the foresight participants.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%