1992
DOI: 10.1177/002383099203500206
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On the Role of Perception in Shaping Phonological Assimilation Rules

Abstract: Assimilation of nasals to the place of articulation of following consonants is a common and natural process among the world's languages. Recent phonological theory attributes this naturalness to the postulated geometry of articulatory features and the notion of spreading (McCarthy, 1988). Others view assimilation as a result of perception (Ohala, 1990), or as perceptually tolerated articulatory simplification (Kohler, 1990). Kohler notes that certain consonant classes (such as nasals and stops) are more likely… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

9
64
0

Year Published

2008
2008
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 78 publications
(75 citation statements)
references
References 14 publications
9
64
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These processes are contextually driven sound changes that usually ease articulation; as a result, the segments surrounding the sound change, and the changed segment itself will overlap more with their phonological features (e.g., the final nasal of lean in lean bacon shares place of articulation with the following stop after assimilation). According to some phonological approaches, such phonological rules tend to lead to perceptually inconspicuous changes from the canonical form (e.g., Hura, Lindblom, & Diehl, 1992;Steriade, 2001). Although the frequency and the distribution of variant forms need to be considered in addition to perceptual changes (e.g., Ranbom et al, 2009), it can be said that the most common changes in L1 are not easily noticed.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These processes are contextually driven sound changes that usually ease articulation; as a result, the segments surrounding the sound change, and the changed segment itself will overlap more with their phonological features (e.g., the final nasal of lean in lean bacon shares place of articulation with the following stop after assimilation). According to some phonological approaches, such phonological rules tend to lead to perceptually inconspicuous changes from the canonical form (e.g., Hura, Lindblom, & Diehl, 1992;Steriade, 2001). Although the frequency and the distribution of variant forms need to be considered in addition to perceptual changes (e.g., Ranbom et al, 2009), it can be said that the most common changes in L1 are not easily noticed.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The fundamental assumption is that some sounds are perceptually more salient than others, such that the perceptually more robust sounds tend not to undergo phonological alterations for the listeners' benefit whereas perceptually weaker sounds tend to be phonologically modified, driven by the principle of ease of articulation. (See also Cho and McQueen (2008) and Hura, Lindblom, and Diehl (1992) for evidence for differential perceptual stability among different manners of articulations in line with this assumption.) Mitterer and colleagues extended this concept to include context effects: If a segment is difficult to be perceived in a given context, that segment is likely to be assimilated.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…Note that the perceptual-integration account does not require assimilations to be incomplete; it only assumes that the assimilation has minimal perceptual consequences (cf. Hura et al, 1992). so that the assimilated version sounds very similar to the unassimilated version with the latter being perceptually integrated with the percept of the following contextual sound.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…How listeners recover the intended meaning from reduced forms has been a major focus of research in psycholinguistics, especially during the last decade, bridging the gap between the fields of spoken-word recognition and speech perception (e.g., Ernestus, Baayen, & Schreuder, 2002;Gaskell & Marslen-Wilson, 1996Gow, 2001Gow, , 2002Gow & Im, 2004;Hura, Lindblom, & Diehl, 1992;Kemps, Ernestus, Schreuder, & Baayen, 2004;Lahiri & Reetz, 2002;Mitterer & Blomert, 2003;Mitterer, Csépe, Honbolygo, & Blomert, 2006;Mitterer & Ernestus, 2006). Most studies focused on the perception of assimilated forms and converged on two findings.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Auditory processes play an important role in theories of speech production that allow perception to influence production (see, e.g., Boersma, 1998;de Boer, 2000;Hume & Johnson, 2001;Hura et al, 1992;Kohler, 1990;Lindblom, 1990;Mitterer, Csépe, Honbolygo, et al, 2006;Ohala, 1990;Schwartz, Boë, Valleé, & Abry, 1997;Steriade, 2001). In this view, listeners and speakers have conflicting interests: listeners prefer unreduced word forms, which facilitate word recognition, while speakers like to reduce as much as possible in order to minimize articulatory effort (Lindblom, 1990).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%