2018
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01032
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On the Reliability of Switching Costs Across Time and Domains

Abstract: Bilingual speakers are suggested to use control processes to avoid linguistic interference from the unintended language. It is debated whether these bilingual language control (BLC) processes are an instantiation of the more domain-general executive control (EC) processes. Previous studies inconsistently report correlations between measures of linguistic and non-linguistic control in bilinguals. In the present study, we investigate the extent to which there is cross-talk between these two domains of control fo… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
37
2

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(41 citation statements)
references
References 63 publications
2
37
2
Order By: Relevance
“…We can therefore neither confirm nor refute the idea that naming pictures in the non-dominant language (as opposed to the dominant language) draws on domain-general cognitive control mechanisms. Similar to previous research, both on bilingual children ( Gross and Kaushanskaya, 2016 ) and adults ( Calabria et al, 2011 , 2015 ; Branzi et al, 2016 ), our study did not find direct relationships between the processing costs (switching/mixing costs) caused by language switching and nonverbal task-switching, although there are also studies that report relationships between language switching and task switching with regard to these measures ( Declerck et al, 2017 ; Timmer et al, 2018 ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…We can therefore neither confirm nor refute the idea that naming pictures in the non-dominant language (as opposed to the dominant language) draws on domain-general cognitive control mechanisms. Similar to previous research, both on bilingual children ( Gross and Kaushanskaya, 2016 ) and adults ( Calabria et al, 2011 , 2015 ; Branzi et al, 2016 ), our study did not find direct relationships between the processing costs (switching/mixing costs) caused by language switching and nonverbal task-switching, although there are also studies that report relationships between language switching and task switching with regard to these measures ( Declerck et al, 2017 ; Timmer et al, 2018 ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Neuroimaging work indicates that the language control network, which subserves control operations when bilinguals produce (Abutalebi & Green, 2008Luk, Green, Abutalebi, & Grady, 2012) and comprehend language (Peeters, Vanlangendonck, Rueschemeyer, & Dijkstra, 2019), overlaps with the network implicated in executive control more generally (e.g., Abutalebi et al, 2011;Blanco-Elorrieta & Pylkkänen, 2016, 2017Branzi, Della Rosa, Canini, Costa, & Abutalebi, 2016;De Baene, Duyck, Brass, & Carreiras, 2015;De Bruin et al, 2014;Coderre, Smith, Van Heuven, & Horwitz, 2016;Garbin et al, 2010;Grant, Fang, & Li, 2015;Green & Abutalebi, 2013). The exact degree of overlap, however, is under debate and views differ in whether they posit only partial overlap (e.g., Calabria, Baus, & Costa, 2019;Magezi, Khateb, Mouthon, Spierer, & Annoni, 2012;Timmer, Calabria, Branzi, Baus, & Costa, 2018;Weissberger, Wierenga, Bondi, & Gollan, 2012) or significant overlap between highly similar brain circuits (e.g., Abutalebi & Green, 2016;De Baene et al, 2015;Pliatsikas & Luk, 2016).…”
Section: Switching Languages Versus Switching Listenersmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Executive control and language control are broad notions that involve a variety of subcomponents, including goal maintenance, conflict monitoring, interference suppression, cue detection, response inhibition, and task (dis)engagement (Calabria, Costa, Green, & Abutalebi, 2018;Green & Abutalebi, 2013). It has traditionally been difficult to reliably compare non-linguistic executive control processes to linguistic control processes in the same bilingual population in tasks that are matched in all respects except for the involvement of language (Calabria et al, 2018;De Baene et al, 2015;Linck, Schwieter, & Sunderman, 2012;Soveri, Rodriguez-Fornells, & Laine, 2011;Timmer et al, 2018). The present study tackles this issue from a novel angle, by comparing instances of bilingual language production that differ in the number of task set reconfigurations necessary.…”
Section: Switching Languages Versus Switching Listenersmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The SWITCH COST refers to slower response latencies for switch trials, where a picture is named in the opposite language from that used in the previous trial, compared to trials in which the same language is used -nonswitch trials (e.g., Calabria, Hernández, Branzi & Costa, 2011;Costa, Santesteban & Ivanova, 2006;Jackson, Swainson, Cunnington & Jackson, 2001;Meuter & Allport, 1999;Philipp, Gade & Koch, 2007;Schwieter & Sunderman, 2008;Timmer, Calabria, Branzi, Baus & Costa, 2018;Verhoef, Roelofs & Chwilla, 2009). Interestingly, the magnitude of the switch cost for each language (L1 vs. L2) seems to depend on the speaker's proficiency level.…”
Section: Language Switching Patterns: the Switch Costmentioning
confidence: 99%