2002
DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.4.947
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On being sad and mistaken: Mood effects on the accuracy of thin-slice judgments.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
157
1
6

Year Published

2005
2005
2012
2012

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 233 publications
(168 citation statements)
references
References 120 publications
(224 reference statements)
4
157
1
6
Order By: Relevance
“…Sad individuals, in contrast, are more likely to ignore heuristic shortcuts and to use effortful, vigilant processing even when it is not required, and therefore perform poorly in complex and timelimited situations (Gleicher & Weary, 1991). For example, in a recent study, sad participants displayed reduced accuracy of social judgments based on thin slices of nonverbal behavior (Ambady & Gray, 2002). The authors showed that sadness impaired accuracy by promoting a deliberative style of processing information, which can interfere with a person's ability to understand others.…”
Section: Performance On Complex Mental Tasksmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Sad individuals, in contrast, are more likely to ignore heuristic shortcuts and to use effortful, vigilant processing even when it is not required, and therefore perform poorly in complex and timelimited situations (Gleicher & Weary, 1991). For example, in a recent study, sad participants displayed reduced accuracy of social judgments based on thin slices of nonverbal behavior (Ambady & Gray, 2002). The authors showed that sadness impaired accuracy by promoting a deliberative style of processing information, which can interfere with a person's ability to understand others.…”
Section: Performance On Complex Mental Tasksmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It looks for evidence of three possible response patterns. The first is that sadness triggers more causal attributions than does anger, due to the heightened level of active, deliberate processing associated with sadness (Alloy & Abramson, 1979;Ambady & Gray, 2002;Bodenhausen, 2000;Bodenhausen et al, 1994;Gleicher & Weary, 1991;Wenzlaff et al, 1988). The second, and contradictory, pattern is that sadness creates fewer causal attributions than anger, because anger arises from and gives rise to appraisals of justice and blame (Lazarus, 1991;Lerner et al, 1998).…”
Section: Synthesis Of Present Study and Hypothesesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Sadness and anger have been studied, however, in other relevant aspects of social information processing. For example, sadness triggers active, deliberative thought (Alloy & Abramson, 1979;Ambady & Gray, 2002;Bodenhausen, Gabriel, & Lineberger, 2000;Gleicher & Weary, 1991;Wenzlaff, Wegner, & Roper, 1988), whereas anger triggers relatively heuristic thought (Lerner, Goldberg, & Tetlock, 1998;Tiedens & Linton, 2001). In a direct comparison of these two negative emotions, sad people used more systematic, detail-oriented strategies than did angry people, including relying less on stereotypes and other heuristic cues (Bodenhausen, Sheppard, & Kramer, 1994).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Note also that negative emotions do not necessarily inspire greater accuracy: sadness has been shown to impair the accuracy of various judgments (Ambady & Gray, 2002).…”
Section: Team Mental Models Team Performance and Team Outcomesmentioning
confidence: 99%