1959
DOI: 10.1037/h0046068
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Observational learning in the rhesus monkey.

Abstract: Numerous attempts have been made to demonstrate that species of animals other than man are capable of learning by observation or imitation. An especially important study for our present purposes is that by Crawford and Spence (1939), who, using two opposing cages with a stimulus tray between them, trained a chimpanzee to observe another depress one of three stimulus objects. After several trials by the demonstrator, the observer was given a test trial to determine if the observer would depress the same object.… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

1
32
1

Year Published

1965
1965
2001
2001

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 87 publications
(34 citation statements)
references
References 13 publications
1
32
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The data further suggest that, when later required to find that target, individuals who have received such training remember the location of the target and move more directly to it than do naive individuals. These data are consistent with similar investigations of rats in the Morris water maze (see, e.g., Keith & McVety, 1988;Sutherland & Linggard, 1982) and of animals in real-world space (see, e.g., Bednekoff & Balda, 1996;Darby & Riopelle, 1959;Heyes & Dawson, 1990;Heyes, Jaldow, & Dawson, 1994;Zentall, Sutton, & Sherburne, 1996). The data are also consistent with predictions made by cognitive mapping theory (O'Keefe & Nadel, 1978).…”
Section: Summary Of Results Of Experiments 1-3supporting
confidence: 89%
“…The data further suggest that, when later required to find that target, individuals who have received such training remember the location of the target and move more directly to it than do naive individuals. These data are consistent with similar investigations of rats in the Morris water maze (see, e.g., Keith & McVety, 1988;Sutherland & Linggard, 1982) and of animals in real-world space (see, e.g., Bednekoff & Balda, 1996;Darby & Riopelle, 1959;Heyes & Dawson, 1990;Heyes, Jaldow, & Dawson, 1994;Zentall, Sutton, & Sherburne, 1996). The data are also consistent with predictions made by cognitive mapping theory (O'Keefe & Nadel, 1978).…”
Section: Summary Of Results Of Experiments 1-3supporting
confidence: 89%
“…Observational learning has already been shown in birds [Klopfer, 1957;Sexton and Finch, 1967] cats [Adler, 1955;John et al, 1968] and also in primates [Darby and Riopelle, 1959;Feldman and Klopfer, 1972], but the problems presented to the animals varied considerably so that the results are not always comparable. It was nevertheless evident that the demonstra tion experience has been efficiently used by observers in all the studied species.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…This manipulation is referred to as vicarious reinforcement. Although there is evidence for the role of vicarious reinforcement in imitation, it is limited primarily to experiments with humans (Lewis & Duncan, 1958;McBrearty, Marston, & Kanfer, 1961) and rhesus monkeys (Darby & Riopelle, 1959;Warden, Fjeld, & Koch, 1940). For example, Groesbeck and Duerfeldt (1971) did not find a significant effect of the observation of reinforcement of demonstrator rats' behavior.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%