2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.03.016
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Normative expectations about fairness: The development of a charity norm in preschoolers

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

10
56
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 76 publications
(67 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
10
56
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Importantly, differences were also found in children's judgments of others’ allocation decisions. These results extend past literature examining children's judgments of equal, equitable, and meritorious allocations (Rizzo et al., ; Schmidt et al., ; Wörle & Paulus, ) by documenting children's early ability to distinguish between the fairness or unfairness of these allocation strategies in response to explicitly individually and structurally based inequalities. Given children's early preference for strict equality, it is important to know when children begin to differentiate between different forms of unequal allocations.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 82%
“…Importantly, differences were also found in children's judgments of others’ allocation decisions. These results extend past literature examining children's judgments of equal, equitable, and meritorious allocations (Rizzo et al., ; Schmidt et al., ; Wörle & Paulus, ) by documenting children's early ability to distinguish between the fairness or unfairness of these allocation strategies in response to explicitly individually and structurally based inequalities. Given children's early preference for strict equality, it is important to know when children begin to differentiate between different forms of unequal allocations.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 82%
“…Different studies demonstrate that preschoolers have a preference to allocate more resources (between other people) and share at greater levels (between self and others) with resource-poor, rather than resource-rich recipients [43,44,45]. Alongside these behavioural findings, research also suggests that older preschoolers have normative expectations for more charitable distributions to poor, rather than rich recipients [46]. Thus, any preference to share more with the puppet that was awarded with stickers is unlikely to be influenced by an associationist strategy, but rather reflects the levels of competence demonstrated by the high merit puppet co-worker.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Such reasoning (e.g., thinking about what the puppet would think about them) involves more advanced forms of social understanding (e.g., second order false belief), and is considered to be beyond the capabilities of pre-schoolers [47]. However, pre-schoolers are sensitive to social cues (e.g., the presence of watching eyes) and modify their levels of generosity in such contexts [44,45,46]. This suggests that three-and four-year-olds within the present study, at least implicitly, are able to make inferences about how their co-worker puppet would view their sharing behaviour.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In this task, we opted to use puppets to ensure that our stimuli did not inadvertently contain any gender, racial, or socioeconomic cues. Prior work has found that children are willing to engage in a host of behaviors towards puppets, including sharing (Chernyak & Kushnir, ), protesting and correcting (Rackozy, Warneken, & Tomasello, ), making moral evaluations (Hamlin, ), engaging in punishment (Worle & Paulus, ), and enforcing norms of fair sharing (Rakoczy et al., ).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%