1990
DOI: 10.1207/s15328023top1704_10
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Negative Reinforcement and Positive Punishment

Abstract: The concept of negative reinforcement is notoriously difficult to teach to introductory students. Reasons for this difficulty include: surplus meanings associated with the terms negative and punishment, the fact that Skinner (1938, 1953) described two types of reinforcement but only one type of punishment, the tendency that most students have to view reward and punishment in subjective terms, and Skinner's (1938, 1953) conflicting definitions of reinforcement and punishment. Pointing out to students that there… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2004
2004
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 5 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Reinforcement is produced by denying the subject the opportunity to occupy a pleasant state as long as it would choose to, thus strengthening instrumental responding to approach or maintain that pleasant state; whereas punishment is produced by forcing the subject to occupy an unpleasant state longer than it would choose to, thus suppressing instrumental responding to avoid or escape from an unpleasant state (Estes, 1944;Solomon, 1964). A key insight arising from this distinction is that an aversive outcome can either reinforce (i.e., strengthen) or punish (i.e., weaken) an instrumentally conditioned response, depending on the context by which that outcome is presented (Crosbie, 1998;McConnell, 1990;Terhune & Premack, 1974). See Figure 1b for illustration.…”
Section: Motivational Contextmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Reinforcement is produced by denying the subject the opportunity to occupy a pleasant state as long as it would choose to, thus strengthening instrumental responding to approach or maintain that pleasant state; whereas punishment is produced by forcing the subject to occupy an unpleasant state longer than it would choose to, thus suppressing instrumental responding to avoid or escape from an unpleasant state (Estes, 1944;Solomon, 1964). A key insight arising from this distinction is that an aversive outcome can either reinforce (i.e., strengthen) or punish (i.e., weaken) an instrumentally conditioned response, depending on the context by which that outcome is presented (Crosbie, 1998;McConnell, 1990;Terhune & Premack, 1974). See Figure 1b for illustration.…”
Section: Motivational Contextmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, a classic rodent study found that the same aversive activity (i.e., forced running) strengthened leverpressing in one component of a multiple schedule, but suppressed drinking behavior in another component (Terhune & Premack, 1974). Importantly, this pivotal revelation demonstrates that the same aversive activity or outcome can reinforce (i.e., strengthen) or punish (i.e., weaken) an instrumental response, depending on the context by which the activity or outcome is presented (Crosbie, 1998;McConnell, 1990).…”
Section: Aversive Outcomes May Strengthen or Weaken Behavioral Responses Depending On The Motivational Contextmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Terminology surrounding basic equine learning theory is commonly misconstrued, with positive and negative being taken with an emotional value either relating to the appropriateness of the stimulus or of the reaction (McConnell, 1990;Tauber, 1988).…”
Section: Knowledge and Application Of Learning Theory In Uk-based Equmentioning
confidence: 99%