1974
DOI: 10.2307/1127752
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Logical Abilities of Young Children -- Two Styles of Approach

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
11
0
1

Year Published

1976
1976
2002
2002

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
11
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Most studies show a clear tendency for children as young as 7 years of age who reason with meaningful premises to produce a biconditional response pattern (e.g. Knifong, 1974;Wildman & Fletcher, 1977). There is some evidence that children can produce the pattern of responses that corresponds to use of the initial model (Barrouillet & Lecas, 1998) although only with unfamiliar premises.…”
Section: Please Scroll Down For Articlementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Most studies show a clear tendency for children as young as 7 years of age who reason with meaningful premises to produce a biconditional response pattern (e.g. Knifong, 1974;Wildman & Fletcher, 1977). There is some evidence that children can produce the pattern of responses that corresponds to use of the initial model (Barrouillet & Lecas, 1998) although only with unfamiliar premises.…”
Section: Please Scroll Down For Articlementioning
confidence: 99%
“…For a given "If P then Q" relation, there are three categories of information that can modify the relations between an antecedent and a given consequent. These are (1) cases of <not-p and q> that have been shown to influence responding to the two invalid forms (Cummins, 1995;Cummins, 1995;Markovits & Vachon, 1990), (2) cases of <p and not-q> that influence responding to MP and MT (Cummins, 1995;Cummins et al, 1991;JanveauBrennan & Markovits, 1999), (3) cases of <not-p and not-q> that are necessary for explaining the strong tendency for biconditional responding in both children and adults (Knifong, 1974;Wildman & Fletcher, 1977). If this is the case, then Downloaded by [Selcuk Universitesi] at 07:59 27 December 2014 the high proportion of correct responding observed in young children for certain premise contents can be accounted for if we assume (a) in line with Andrews and Halford (1998) that young children can manipulate two relations (models) in working memory, and (b) that children use the minor premise as a retrieval cue during the fleshing-out process.…”
Section: Please Scroll Down For Articlementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Adolescents also do poorly on conversion, as shown in table 3. Knifong (1974) attempts to provide what he regards as a Piagetian explanation of the successes children have on transitivity and contraposition, suggesting that their thinking could be interpreted as transduction rather than deduction. He also feels that this would explain their poor performance on the invalidity principles.…”
Section: A Broad View Of Empirical Results Utilizing the Proposed Conmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Finally, it could be argued that some of the retardates' explanations support Knifong's (1974) conjecture as to why children frequently interpret a conditional premise (i.e., If P, then Q) as a biconditional (i.e., P if and only if Q). Knifong demonstrated that such interpretations can be explained by the Piagetian construct of transductive reasoning; that is, the children reason that P and Q must co-occur.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%