1992
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.18.3.452
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Lexical and sublexical translation of spelling to sound: Strategic anticipation of lexical status.

Abstract: Two experiments on oral reading of single words compared naming performance in pure blocks of nonwords or exception words with performance in blocks of randomly mixed nonwords and exception words. Ss named exception words faster and made fewer regularization errors when they were not also prepared for nonwords. These data suggest Ss inhibit or ignore the computation of assembled phonology when only exception words are expected. Ss named nonwords faster, but no more accurately, when low-frequency exception word… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

26
269
2
3

Year Published

1995
1995
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 190 publications
(301 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
26
269
2
3
Order By: Relevance
“…If this character-by-character reading for the nonword stimuli were to induce a similar strategy for the words, then-because both word and character levels would yield only one possible pronunciation for the consistent words but could yield conflicting pronunciations for the inconsistent words-wnsistency effects in Kanji reading might emerge. The parallel to this predicted phenomenon in English has been demonstrated by Monsell, Patterson, Graham, Hughes, and Milroy (1992), who found a significantly higher error rate (particularly regularization errors) to exceptional inconsistent English words when nonwords were mixed in with the words.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 73%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…If this character-by-character reading for the nonword stimuli were to induce a similar strategy for the words, then-because both word and character levels would yield only one possible pronunciation for the consistent words but could yield conflicting pronunciations for the inconsistent words-wnsistency effects in Kanji reading might emerge. The parallel to this predicted phenomenon in English has been demonstrated by Monsell, Patterson, Graham, Hughes, and Milroy (1992), who found a significantly higher error rate (particularly regularization errors) to exceptional inconsistent English words when nonwords were mixed in with the words.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 73%
“…It would of course be impossible to offer a single value for a lexicality effect in experiments with English readers, as the effect will clearly vary as a function of the characteristics of the words, the nonwords, the reading skill of the participants, and so forth. Several studies in the literature, however, in which English words and matched nonwords were named in mixed lists (as in the design of Experiment 4 for Kanji) suggest a typical R T difference in the region of 30-50 ms (see, for example, Glushko, 1979;Monsell et al, 1992 The idea that processing in Kanji relies primarily on a whole-word level finds some general support in a study by Morton, Sasanuma, Patterson, and Sakuma (1992), who investigated recognition units for Kanji words by using a different paradigm to the current naming experiments. In that study, target words to be identified tachistoscopicaliy were preceded, about half an hour earlier and in a different task, by words in one of several critical conditions: (a) an identity prime (the prime and target words were identical), (b) a single-character prime (the target was a two-character word, and the prime was one of its component characters), and (c) a two-character prime (in which the target was either a single-character word corresponding to one of the components of the prime or a different two-character word sharing a character with the prime).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Several studies suggest that participants can adopt different time criteria when to respond contingent on task difficulty (Lupker, Brown, & Colombo, 1997;Monsell, Patterson, Graham, Hughes, & Milroy, 1992;Taylor & Lupker, 2001). For example, Lupker et al (1997) found that participants can adjust their performance in relation to the preceding trial.…”
Section: Disyllabic Pseudo-words With Frequency Manipulated Syllablesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The impact of lexical information on phoneme processing appears to be modulated by the degree of attention to lexical information ( [52][53][54] and similar effects have been found on speech production [55] and reading [56]). Norris et al [4] argued that to account for this attentional modulation, interactive models would have to turn off feedback, thereby making them autonomous.…”
Section: Attentional Modulation Of Lexical Effectsmentioning
confidence: 99%