2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2018.04.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learning homophones in context: Easy cases are favored in the lexicon of natural languages

Abstract: Even though ambiguous words are common in languages, children find it hard to learn homophones, where a single label applies to several distinct meanings (e.g., Mazzocco, 1997). The present work addresses this apparent discrepancy between learning abilities and typological pattern, with respect to homophony in the lexicon. In a series of five experiments, 20-month-old French children easily learnt a pair of homophones if the two meanings associated with the phonological form belonged to different syntactic cat… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
44
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(44 citation statements)
references
References 62 publications
0
44
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This suggests that the pressure is not against the existence of homophony per se, but rather, could reflect a constraint on the extent to which any given wordform can be saturated with distinct, unrelated meanings. Assigning too many unrelated meanings to the same signal could impede communication or learning (Casenhiser, 2005; though see Dautriche et al, 2018), and may thus be selected against. Such a pressure against oversaturation is roughly analogous to what others have termed diversification (Zipf, 1949) or a pressure for clarity (Piantadosi et al, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This suggests that the pressure is not against the existence of homophony per se, but rather, could reflect a constraint on the extent to which any given wordform can be saturated with distinct, unrelated meanings. Assigning too many unrelated meanings to the same signal could impede communication or learning (Casenhiser, 2005; though see Dautriche et al, 2018), and may thus be selected against. Such a pressure against oversaturation is roughly analogous to what others have termed diversification (Zipf, 1949) or a pressure for clarity (Piantadosi et al, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such a model should also predict which potential homophones will be selected against (and what form this selection process takes, i.e., whether it is via the avoidance of homophony-inducing mergers (Wedel et al, 2013;Yin & White, 2018) or something else) and which will be preserved. Homophones should be more likely to survive in a lexicon if their meanings are systematically made sufficiently discriminable by context (Dautriche et al, 2018). A better understanding of this process would also yield insights into which sources of contextual information human speakers and comprehenders routinely sample and deploy for disambiguation, and therefore influence language change.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…", and conclude that communication features in the design of language as a cognitive system. The PTG study has been influential (see also Dautriche, Fibla, Fievet, and Christophe (2018); Mahowald, Fedorenko, Piantadosi, and Gibson (2013); Piantadosi, Tily, and Gibson (2011) for similar studies), and the communicative function served by context, which is critical for PTG's study of lexical ambiguity, has been extended to other domains of language including morphology and syntax (see Gibson et al 2019 for a review).…”
Section: Correlating Ambiguity With Communicative Efficiencymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Appendix for the items), because no distractor is available. No feedback is given to the participants to validate their interpretation after their choice, in any item, induction or test, as was the case in other similar previous studies(Havron et al, 2019;Yurovsky et al, 2016 andDautriche et al, 2018).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%