2020
DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/k5tbm
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

4-5-year-old children adapt to the reliability of conflicting sources of information to learn novel words

Abstract: A central challenge in language acquisition is the integration of multiple sources of information, potentially in conflict, to acquire new knowledge and adjust current linguistic representations. One way to accomplish this is to assign more weight to more reliable sources of information in context. We test the hypothesis that children adjust the weight of different sources of information during learning, considering two specific sources of information: their knowledge of the meaning of familiar words (semantic… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
1
1

Relationship

1
1

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 4 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…With that said, recent years have seen multiple publications describing weaker prediction or prediction adaptation the younger the participants (Barrault, Havron, Dautriche, Babineau, de Carvalho, Christophe, in preparation ; Beretti et al, 2020; Gambi, Gorrie, Pickering, & Rabagliati, 2018; Havron et al, 2019). Furthermore, recent studies have failed to replicate high‐profile adult studies showing fine‐grained syntactic adaptation (Harrington Stack et al, 2018; Liu, Burchill, Tanenhaus, & Jaeger, 2017).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…With that said, recent years have seen multiple publications describing weaker prediction or prediction adaptation the younger the participants (Barrault, Havron, Dautriche, Babineau, de Carvalho, Christophe, in preparation ; Beretti et al, 2020; Gambi, Gorrie, Pickering, & Rabagliati, 2018; Havron et al, 2019). Furthermore, recent studies have failed to replicate high‐profile adult studies showing fine‐grained syntactic adaptation (Harrington Stack et al, 2018; Liu, Burchill, Tanenhaus, & Jaeger, 2017).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such top-down cues include lexical-semantic information about specific words (e.g., verb restrictions, MacDonald, Pearlmutter & Seidenberg, 1994), knowledge about the real world (e.g., action-goal relations, Kamide, Altmann & Haywood, 2003), and pragmatic inferences about speakers' intentions, which are often related to and modulated by the broader discourse context (Nieuwland & Van Berkum, 2006;Otten & Van Berkum, 2008;Tylén et al, 2015). While the relative contribution of bottom-up vs. top-down information in language processing has been the subject of longstanding debate (e.g., Bates & MacWhinney, 1987;Christiansen & Chater, 2016;Kintsch, 2005;Marslen-Wilson & Welsh, 1978;Roark, 2001), recent work suggests that top-down processes become especially relevant when the quality of acoustic-phonetic information decreases (e.g., Beretti et al, 2020;Chodroff & Wilson, 2020;Szostak & Pitt, 2013;Bushong & Jaeger, 2017;Borsky, Tuller & Shapiro, 1998;Gaskell & Marslen-Wilson, 2001). Motivated by recent evidence from Danish and Norwegian (Ishkhanyan et al, 2019;Trecca et al, 2019), we here explore whether the balance between bottom-up and top-down processes might also be affected by cross-linguistic differences between these two languages.…”
Section: Bottom-up and Top-down Processingmentioning
confidence: 99%