2001
DOI: 10.1080/09575140120057194
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Language Development at Home and School: gains and losses in young bilinguals

Abstract: In this paper we examine the topic of the language development of three young bilingual children at both home and school. Our aim is to consider the language issues that arise in such children, taking into account their parents' language background and experience of school systems, language practices and 'policy' in the home, and the children's experience of a year of nursery school as reported by their teachers. We try to show how different strands of the children's experience t together; their 'mother tongue… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

1
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 8 publications
(8 reference statements)
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Parental language choice in bilingual families has been studied from many different perspectives and using different methodologies (compare, for instance, Curdt-Christiansen 2009, De Houwer 2007, Lanza 1997, Li Wei 1994 and Zhu Hua 2008). All of these perspectives can be subsumed under the heading of "Family Language Policy" or FLP (King, Fogle and Logan-Terry 2008, Lanza 2014, Parke and Drury 2001). The present study aims to contribute to FLP by considering the degree to which parental language choice in addressing young bilingual children varies across time or fundamentally remains the same.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Parental language choice in bilingual families has been studied from many different perspectives and using different methodologies (compare, for instance, Curdt-Christiansen 2009, De Houwer 2007, Lanza 1997, Li Wei 1994 and Zhu Hua 2008). All of these perspectives can be subsumed under the heading of "Family Language Policy" or FLP (King, Fogle and Logan-Terry 2008, Lanza 2014, Parke and Drury 2001). The present study aims to contribute to FLP by considering the degree to which parental language choice in addressing young bilingual children varies across time or fundamentally remains the same.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This notion of complexity is common in much of the literature concerning FLA and SLA; Soderman and Oshio (, p. 298) consider FLA “complex”, and Clark writes that “second‐language acquisition is as complex as the acquisition of the first language but with a wide variety of variables added in” (Clark, , p. 184), this being one factor leading, presumably, to the “complexity of early years pedagogy” (Bligh, , p. 12). Yet the nature of FLA, and the theories associated with it, are sometimes seen as unproblematic for understanding SLA; Parke and Drury, for example, merely note that a “full, ‘normal’ linguistic environment … is essential for the development of language in young children” (Parke & Drury, , p. 125), leading them to the conjecture that “it does look as though there is a common storage of languages in the mind” (Parke & Drury, , p. 126). Another example which sees the relationship between FLA and SLA as largely unproblematic comes from Clarke (), who writes:
The first language … forms the foundation for all later language development (Clarke, , p. 9).
Similarly, in relation to language acquisition, Soderman and Oshio write:
The process of becoming competent in a first language requires very young children to master: phonology (the sounds of the language); vocabulary (the words of the language); grammar (the way the words are ordered and put together); discourse (the way the sentences are put together); and pragmatics (the rules of how to use the language) … Though complex, a child's initial foray into language development begins at birth as the child interacts with others, building both a receptive vocabulary and a phenomenal ability to express all of the other important pieces of their language in a fairly competent manner by the age of five (Soderman & Oshio, , p. 298).
Quite how children do ‘master’ phonology, grammar, discourse and pragmatics is left unexplained.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Many factors may or may not have an effect upon the speed at which a child passes through the silent period, including the consequences of psychological withdrawal or an interruption in the child's expected 'language acquisition processes' (Parke and Drury, 2001). Kagan (1989) suggests that children who are temperamentally inhibited will be more cautious, less sociable and perhaps less willing to try; they may be fearful (with no one to share their mother tongue) of making a mistake, therefore prolonging the transition through the silent period.…”
Section: The Silent Periodmentioning
confidence: 99%