2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.07.012
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

“Lacking warmth”: Alexithymia trait is related to warm-specific thermal somatosensory processing

Abstract: HighlightsAlexithymia scores were related to scores on a quantitative sensory testing battery.Two separate studies found altered low-level somatosensation in alexithymia.High alexithymic participants showed specific reduction in sensitivity to warm temperature.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
16
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 100 publications
1
16
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Indeed, in recent years the HCT has been heavily criticised on the basis that non-interoceptive factors may influence performance (e.g., beliefs, time estimation; Brener & Ring, 2016;Murphy et al, 2018;Ring & Brener, 1996;Ring et al, 2015;Windmann et al, 1999), with concerns also raised regarding the psychometric properties of the task (Zamariola et al, 2018; but see Ainley et al , 2020). Despite these criticisms the HCT remains widely employed, with its use often justified by claiming a moderate correlation with the HDT (e.g., Borhani et al, 2017;Herbert et al, 2013;Pollatos et al, 2007;Scarpazza et al, 2017;Werner et al, 2009), a task that does not suffer from the aforementioned HCT-specific limitations (e.g., Knoll & Hodapp, 1992;Phillips et al, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Indeed, in recent years the HCT has been heavily criticised on the basis that non-interoceptive factors may influence performance (e.g., beliefs, time estimation; Brener & Ring, 2016;Murphy et al, 2018;Ring & Brener, 1996;Ring et al, 2015;Windmann et al, 1999), with concerns also raised regarding the psychometric properties of the task (Zamariola et al, 2018; but see Ainley et al , 2020). Despite these criticisms the HCT remains widely employed, with its use often justified by claiming a moderate correlation with the HDT (e.g., Borhani et al, 2017;Herbert et al, 2013;Pollatos et al, 2007;Scarpazza et al, 2017;Werner et al, 2009), a task that does not suffer from the aforementioned HCT-specific limitations (e.g., Knoll & Hodapp, 1992;Phillips et al, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Similarly, other studies suggest a small but significant correlation between accuracy scores on the tasks (r = .36; Hart, McGowan, Minati, & Critchley, 2013). Such evidence of a small-to-moderate correlation between these measures is often used to justify the use of a single measure of cardiac interoceptive accuracy, as performance is presumed to generalise from one task to the other (e.g., Borhani, Ladavas, Fotopoulou, & Haggard, 2017;Herbert, Blechert, Hautzinger, Matthias, & Herbert, 2013;Pollatos, Traut-Mattausch, Schroeder, & Schandry, 2007;Scarpazza, Sellitto, & di Pellegrino, 2017;Werner et al, 2009). However, there are instances where performance on the HCT and HDT has not been found to correlate; for example, Forkmann et al (2016) found no significant association between performance on the HCT and the 2AFC auditory HDT.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…For example, in experimentally induced pain, participants are asked to identify pinprick pain as 'high' or 'low' based on a predetermined pain threshold (i.e., the moment participant's begin to feel sharp pinprick pain, e.g., Borhani, Làdavas, Fotopoulou, & Haggard, 2017). In contrast, our SDT design employed two thresholds: below and above the CT range.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This suggests that mental simulation for temperature would at least distinguish between cold and warm. Intriguingly, cold and warm sensations are mediated by different afferent fibres, and in thermoception research they are considered distinct submodalities (Borhani, Làdavas, Fotopoulou, & Haggard, 2017).…”
Section: Temperaturementioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, people rate themselves as more similar to another person (greater social proximity) when holding a warm beverage compared to a cold beverage (IJzerman & Semin, 2009), and report feeling more lonely after holding a cold (versus warm or neutral) therapeutic pack (Bargh & Shalev, 2012). However, this research has been treated with some skepticism following failures to replicate (Chabris, Heck, Mandart, Benjamin, & Simons, 2019;Lynott, Corker, Connell, & O'Brien, 2017, 2014, leading researchers to seek alternative paradigms to test the essential premise of a link between physical and social warmth (e.g., Borhani et al, 2017;Fetterman, Wilkowski, & Robinson, 2018). Regardless, such results do not bear directly on the present issue of grounding language of the lower senses, and instead may reflect the association between temperature and social experiences in everyday experience (e.g., the warmth of maternal attachment, the effect of heat on aggression; Lynott et al, 2017) or have a low-level physiological basis (e.g., Borhani et al, 2017).…”
Section: Temperaturementioning
confidence: 99%