2015
DOI: 10.1080/23273798.2015.1072223
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Is prediction necessary to understand language? Probably not

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

14
144
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 179 publications
(160 citation statements)
references
References 109 publications
14
144
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Based on these points, we conclude that such prediction effects are perhaps not in fact representative of how people comprehend language in natural settings. Thus, in concord with Huettig and Mani (2016), we conclude that prediction may not be a necessary computation for language processing to occur, and that, minimally, its role in mechanistic theories of language processing must be carefully evaluated. In some processing architectures (see Martin, 2016), prediction effects would fall out naturally when the right confluences of cues and internal priors were activated, giving rise to an architectural form of predictive coding that, while not necessary for comprehension to occur, may sometimes facilitate processing without the postulation of a separate, active prediction mechanism in the system.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Based on these points, we conclude that such prediction effects are perhaps not in fact representative of how people comprehend language in natural settings. Thus, in concord with Huettig and Mani (2016), we conclude that prediction may not be a necessary computation for language processing to occur, and that, minimally, its role in mechanistic theories of language processing must be carefully evaluated. In some processing architectures (see Martin, 2016), prediction effects would fall out naturally when the right confluences of cues and internal priors were activated, giving rise to an architectural form of predictive coding that, while not necessary for comprehension to occur, may sometimes facilitate processing without the postulation of a separate, active prediction mechanism in the system.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As it's rainy, it's better to go out with a big umbrella). In our view, in natural language settings, articles may not be very reliable cues to upcoming nouns, which means that pre-activation of word form may not be a common phenomenon (for similar arguments, Huettig, 2015;Huettig & Mani, 2016).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…2 For example, when hearing The boy will eat the…, listeners look more at edible than inedible objects in a visual array, and this preference emerges before they hear the noun (…cake) (Altmann & Kamide, 1999). There is good 2 While the extent of the role that prediction plays in language processing is under current debate (Huettig & Mani, 2016;Nieuwland et al, 2017), the contribution of a proactive component to human cognition is widely agreed upon (Bar, 2007).…”
Section: She…) Is Preferentially Interpreted As Referring To the Goalmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although predictive effects triggered by gender information on articles and adjectives have been observed in many languages, e. g., French (Dahan et al 2000), Spanish (Grüter et al 2012;LewWilliams and Fernald 2007), Russian (Sekerina and Trueswell 2011), German (Hopp 2016), and Dutch (Loerts et al 2013), this effect can be modulated by the population tested, the morpho-syntactic system of the language, and the experimental set-up. Children and older adults, speakers with low working memory capacity, and L2 learners show smaller or no signs of anticipation of upcoming language compared to monolingual young controls (Huettig and Mani 2016). Furthermore, morpho-syntactic properties of the language also determine whether an anticipation effect is found.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%