2020
DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1551
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Infants' performance in the indirect false belief tasks: A second‐person interpretation

Abstract: Research in the last 15 years has challenged the idea that false belief attribution develops at 4 years of age. Studies with indirect false belief tasks contend to provide evidence of false belief attribution in the second year of life. We review the literature on indirect false belief tasks carried out in infants using looking and active helping paradigms. Although the results are heterogeneous and not conclusive, such tasks appear to capture a real effect. However, it is misleading to call them “false belief… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 125 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Moreover, denying such precocious representations to preverbal children dissolves the apparent mystery of how preverbal children allegedly discriminate others' false belief in their own gazing behavior, by looking where a person might falsely believe an object to be hidden (e.g., Onishi & Baillargeon, 2005), but cannot verbally express this putative insight until well into their fifth years of life (see, e.g., Wellman et al, 2001, for discussion). Concerning claims of infant discriminations of false beliefs, in a recent review published in the present journal, Barone and Gomila (2020) have developed a second-person perspective on mental attribution that they characterize as a non-propositional attribution of mentality to others; mentality, in this perspective, is transparent in the interactive patterns of babies and their caregivers. As they put it, these "indirect false belief tasks" that are administered to preverbal infants do not require the attribution of a belief state to pass the tasks.…”
Section: Bifurcated Representational Accounts Of Pointingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, denying such precocious representations to preverbal children dissolves the apparent mystery of how preverbal children allegedly discriminate others' false belief in their own gazing behavior, by looking where a person might falsely believe an object to be hidden (e.g., Onishi & Baillargeon, 2005), but cannot verbally express this putative insight until well into their fifth years of life (see, e.g., Wellman et al, 2001, for discussion). Concerning claims of infant discriminations of false beliefs, in a recent review published in the present journal, Barone and Gomila (2020) have developed a second-person perspective on mental attribution that they characterize as a non-propositional attribution of mentality to others; mentality, in this perspective, is transparent in the interactive patterns of babies and their caregivers. As they put it, these "indirect false belief tasks" that are administered to preverbal infants do not require the attribution of a belief state to pass the tasks.…”
Section: Bifurcated Representational Accounts Of Pointingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…See also Barone et al. (2019), Barone and Gomila (2020), Newen and Wolf (2020), and Phillips and Norby (2019).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“… Cp. Spaulding (2020, p. 1).37 On humans, seeBaillargeon et al (2018),Barone et al (2019),Barone and Gomila (2020),Burge (2018),Dudley (2018),Newen and Wolf (2020),Phillips and Norby (2019), Poulin-Dubois et al (2018),Scott and Baillargeon (2017), and the references therein. On non-human animals, seeHorschler et al (2020),Krupenye and Call (2019, § 2.2-2.3), and the references therein.38 For criticism of Burge's interpretation, seeCarruthers (2020) andJacob (2020).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations