2008
DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.1310598
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Identifying Social Norms using Coordination Games: Why Does Dictator Game Sharing Vary?

Abstract: We introduce a method for identifying social norms, based on the property that they reflect collective perceptions regarding the appropriateness of behaviors. We demonstrate that the norms we elicit, along with a simple model combining concern for normcompliance with utility for money, predict changes in behavior across several variants of the dictator game. Our findings indicate that individuals care about monetary payoffs and the social appropriateness of any action taken. Further, we find that a social norm… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

31
610
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 313 publications
(642 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
31
610
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We combine multiple methods by adding an incentive compatible elicitation protocol to traditional vignette-based survey methods, and combining this with a second attitudinal survey and a second experiment about behavior related to the occupation of many subjects. We adapt the method of Krupka and Weber (2009) to a field setting in a financial services firm, and in so doing also extend the protocol to identify norms across groups in the organizational hierarchy and the beliefs each group has about the normative expectations of the others, as well as separating two different norm constructs (group and personal). We use these data to construct measures of normative alignment (between levels of the hierarchy and between the norms of employees and those of their peers and their leaders) and we correlate these measures with indicators of individual on-the-job behavior that are reported in a survey (such as job satisfaction) and observed in a controlled laboratory experiment (dishonest advice).…”
Section: Related and Existing Workmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…We combine multiple methods by adding an incentive compatible elicitation protocol to traditional vignette-based survey methods, and combining this with a second attitudinal survey and a second experiment about behavior related to the occupation of many subjects. We adapt the method of Krupka and Weber (2009) to a field setting in a financial services firm, and in so doing also extend the protocol to identify norms across groups in the organizational hierarchy and the beliefs each group has about the normative expectations of the others, as well as separating two different norm constructs (group and personal). We use these data to construct measures of normative alignment (between levels of the hierarchy and between the norms of employees and those of their peers and their leaders) and we correlate these measures with indicators of individual on-the-job behavior that are reported in a survey (such as job satisfaction) and observed in a controlled laboratory experiment (dishonest advice).…”
Section: Related and Existing Workmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We follow this literature in distinguishing what one "ought" to do, or injunctive norms, from customs or actions that people regularly take, or descriptive norms (Bicchieri, 2006, Deutsch andGerard, 1955). Both kinds of norms influence behavior (Herrbach and Mignonac 2007;Cialdini et al 1990;Krupka and Weber 2009;Bicchieri and Xiao 2009;Gneezy et al 2010). However, our primary focus in this paper is on injunctive norms, i.e., those described by Elster (1989a) as prescribing what one "should do" or "should not do.…”
Section: A Defining Ethical Normsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations