2009
DOI: 10.1353/aad.0.0071
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Establishment of Joint Attention in Dyads Involving Hearing Mothers of Deaf and Hearing Children, and Its Relation to Adaptive Social Behavior

Abstract: Mounting evidence points to joint attention as a mediating variable in children's adaptive behavior development. Joint attention in interactions between hearing mothers and congenitally deaf ( n = 27) and hearing ( n = 29) children, ages 18–36 months, was examined. All deaf children had severe to profound hearing loss. Mother-child interactions were coded for maternally initiated and child-initiated success rates in establishing joint attention; mothers completed ratings of their children's adaptive behavior. … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Particularly in cases where there is little to no sign language used in a hearing-status mismatched parent-child dyad, as is often the case with children who are candidates for cochlear implantation, the establishment of joint attention can serve as an important scaffold for children to learn about communicative intent, as is the case for children in hearing-status matched dyads. Thus far, researchers have focused on hearing-mismatched parent-child dyads to identify strategies used by parents to engage children's attention (Gale & Schick, 2009;Lieberman et al, 2014), characterize parents' adaptive social behaviors (Nowakowski et al, 2009), and compare overall amounts of joint attention across dyad types (i.e., hearing parent-deaf child, hearing parent-hearing child, and deaf parent-deaf child) (Nowakowski et al, 2009;Spencer, 2004;Spencer et al, 1992). Although these studies generally include small sample sizes and children with highly heterogeneous hearing issues, their findings show that while hearing parents are sensitive to deaf children's communicative efforts, the overall rate of maternally initiated joint attention is lower in hearing status-mismatched dyads.…”
Section: Joint Attention In Hearing-status Mismatched Dyadsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Particularly in cases where there is little to no sign language used in a hearing-status mismatched parent-child dyad, as is often the case with children who are candidates for cochlear implantation, the establishment of joint attention can serve as an important scaffold for children to learn about communicative intent, as is the case for children in hearing-status matched dyads. Thus far, researchers have focused on hearing-mismatched parent-child dyads to identify strategies used by parents to engage children's attention (Gale & Schick, 2009;Lieberman et al, 2014), characterize parents' adaptive social behaviors (Nowakowski et al, 2009), and compare overall amounts of joint attention across dyad types (i.e., hearing parent-deaf child, hearing parent-hearing child, and deaf parent-deaf child) (Nowakowski et al, 2009;Spencer, 2004;Spencer et al, 1992). Although these studies generally include small sample sizes and children with highly heterogeneous hearing issues, their findings show that while hearing parents are sensitive to deaf children's communicative efforts, the overall rate of maternally initiated joint attention is lower in hearing status-mismatched dyads.…”
Section: Joint Attention In Hearing-status Mismatched Dyadsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Why is this? The coding scheme employed in the present study is one that we have carefully constructed based on extensive prior research by others (Nowakowski et al, 2009;Tomasello & Farrar, 1986) and arguably captures what researchers intended when joint attention was initially documented and classified (e.g., Bakeman & Adamson, 1984). An issue for consideration in future research is whether episodes of joint attention are coded consistently across different labs.…”
Section: Joint Attention Codingmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In these instances, dyads engage in a free-play session with an adult, uninterrupted by prompting or technology. These interactions are typically recorded and coded offline, incorporating specific criteria for characterizing different components of the interaction, including bouts of joint attention (Bakeman & Adamson, 1984;de Barbaro et al, 2016d;Gale & Schick, 2009;Nowakowski et al, 2009;Salo et al, 2018;Tomasello & Farrar, 1986). The different protocols consist of components intended to provide guidance on codable criteria, with agreement between coders assessed via interrater reliability.…”
Section: Protocols For Assessing Joint Attentionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Here we introduce a coding protocol that integrates components of the different approaches to joint attention that we have outlined. In particular, the protocol is inspired by the work of Tomasello and Farrer (1986) and of Nowakowski et al (2009), with timing details founded on insights from both visual observation (Bakeman & Adamson, 1984) and eye-tracking (Abney et al, 2017;Yu & Smith, 2013). Critically, our approach allows coding of both the initiation of joint attention (by both/either parent and child), and maintenance of joint attention (again, by both members of the dyad).…”
Section: Development Of Coding Protocolmentioning
confidence: 99%