2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2013.12.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of primary and secondary morphological family size in monolingual and bilingual word processing

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
39
0
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 87 publications
(48 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
4
39
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…They emerge not only in the present data set, but have been observed for visual lexical decision (Mulder et al, 2014) as well as for word naming and for eeg data (Baayen et al, 2016b). If this interpretation is correct, penalized factor smooths are the appropriate statistical tool to use.…”
Section: The Kkl Datasetsupporting
confidence: 62%
“…They emerge not only in the present data set, but have been observed for visual lexical decision (Mulder et al, 2014) as well as for word naming and for eeg data (Baayen et al, 2016b). If this interpretation is correct, penalized factor smooths are the appropriate statistical tool to use.…”
Section: The Kkl Datasetsupporting
confidence: 62%
“…Alternatively, it is possible that for some reason specific to the bilingual language architecture, bilinguals would use cognate words overall more often than monolinguals do (Sadat, Pureza, & Alario, unpublished data). Whatever the origin for a confounded bilingual frequency count could be, in this context it is important to mention that in the present data there was no significant interaction between lexical frequency and translation similarity (e.g., see also Costa et al, 2000;Mulder, Dijkstra, Schreuder, & Baayen, 2014). Therefore, the effects of phonological similarity and frequency are assumed to be additive and considered separately in this discussion.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 61%
“…Baayen et al (2011) take the 20 most activated competitors into account as competitors for the target meaning, and Mulder, Dijkstra, Schreuder, and Baayen (2013) show that within the NDR approach, the summed activation of non-targeted meanings above a certain threshold can be taken into account, in line with the multiple readout framework of Grainger and Jacobs (1996). The experience of these studies is that the target's activation is the main determinant of response latencies, with additional mechanisms for conflict resolution providing minor improvements in goodness of fit.…”
Section: Discrimination Learning and The Naive Discriminative Readermentioning
confidence: 88%