2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.10.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Distinguishing the affective and cognitive bases of implicit attitudes to improve prediction of food choices

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

5
35
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 44 publications
(41 citation statements)
references
References 68 publications
5
35
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The results support the majority of the hypotheses, reflecting the overall validity of the proposed attitudinal model. First, the results indicate that, as regards implicit attitudes toward green products, the cognitive component can be differentiated from the affective component, which is consistent with the ABC model of attitudes [94] and the findings reported by Trendel and Werle [47]. This finding implies that the preferences or evaluations that are generated outside cognitive processing also have a rational influence from what has been learnt subconsciously and what has been felt unconsciously.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The results support the majority of the hypotheses, reflecting the overall validity of the proposed attitudinal model. First, the results indicate that, as regards implicit attitudes toward green products, the cognitive component can be differentiated from the affective component, which is consistent with the ABC model of attitudes [94] and the findings reported by Trendel and Werle [47]. This finding implies that the preferences or evaluations that are generated outside cognitive processing also have a rational influence from what has been learnt subconsciously and what has been felt unconsciously.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…Second, unlike in the study by Trendel and Werle [47], a significant effect was only observed for the effect of cognitive implicit attitudes on explicit attitudes. This finding stands to reason because these authors [47] analyzed responses to the choice of certain foods with a strong hedonic focus (e.g., chocolate). However, the green products considered in this study are functional, so the significance of the cognitive focus makes sense.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 77%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In both study cohorts there were no descriptive differences in explicit liking between healthy and unhealthy most liked foods that could transfer to the APP. Although healthiness may not affect priming effects in an explicit manner that would undermine its validity as an indirect measure of liking-as, for example, when these reflect concerns related to health, weight-related goals or social norms (Czyzewska & Graham, 2008)-healthiness could influence task outcomes through implicit healthiness attributes that are automatically retrieved from memory (e.g., see Rangel, 2013;Trendel & Werle, 2015). We specifically assumed that if individuals had greater automatic affective reactions towards unhealthy foods (e.g., "unhealthy = tasty" intuition; Raghunathan, Naylor, & Hoyer, 2006; also see Werle, Trendel, & Ardito, 2013), this would manifest as a positive difference in the magnitude of RT/ER priming effects, even when food primes were matched on explicit liking.…”
Section: Is the App Sensitive To Cognitive Components Of Food Attitudes?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The affective component reflects an individual's hedonic reaction to the sensory properties of foods, commonly referred to as food liking, which is a central determinant of dietary choice (Eertmans, Baeyens, & Van den Bergh, 2001). The cognitive component may involve thoughts about the nutritional value of a food item and potential health consequences (Trendel & Werle, 2015). This study examined the methodological validity of an indirect measure of attitudes-the affective priming paradigm (APP; Fazio & Olson, 2003;Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986;Hermans, De Houwer, & Eelen, 2001;Klauer & Musch, 2003)-and the extent to which priming measures were sensitive to affective (i.e., liking) and cognitive (i.e., healthiness) components of food attitudes.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%