2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2015.11.013
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Differences in children’s thinking and learning during attentional focus instruction

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

1
37
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 34 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
1
37
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Children with moderate VI (i.e., classified as B3 and B4) had less balance error when using an external focus, which supports the constrained action hypothesis by suggesting that when vision is moderately impaired, an external focus can still result in more automated processing. This finding coincides with the widespread results from previous research with typically developing adults and typically developing children ( Wulf et al, 2010 ; Wulf, 2013 ; Perreault and French, 2016 ). In addition to balance error data, previous research involving the same task has reported increased mean power frequency (MPF) (i.e., smaller, more frequent adjustments) while using an external focus of attention ( Wulf et al, 2001a ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…Children with moderate VI (i.e., classified as B3 and B4) had less balance error when using an external focus, which supports the constrained action hypothesis by suggesting that when vision is moderately impaired, an external focus can still result in more automated processing. This finding coincides with the widespread results from previous research with typically developing adults and typically developing children ( Wulf et al, 2010 ; Wulf, 2013 ; Perreault and French, 2016 ). In addition to balance error data, previous research involving the same task has reported increased mean power frequency (MPF) (i.e., smaller, more frequent adjustments) while using an external focus of attention ( Wulf et al, 2001a ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…Although beneficial effects of external focus instruction and feedback have been found for children's motor performance and learning (e.g., Palmer et al 2017), this has not been consistently observed (e.g., Jarus et al, 2015;Perreault & French, 2016). These findings are in line with the constrained action hypothesis (Wulf, McNevin, & Shea, 2001).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 72%
“…Using a within-subjects approach, the present study is unable to clearly address the role of different attentional focus instructions in the acquisition of skills in childhood, and any long-term impact is unclear. Although the researcher checked for instruction comprehension, further manipulation check efforts would have provided information on how the instructions were used (see Perreault & French, 2016). As discussed, it is quite possible that characteristics of intention, distance and the task goals resulted in the external-far conditions being the most usable instructional-set provided as they were supported by the presence of the cone in the task set up.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The handful of studies that were performed in typically developing children have led to equivocal results. Chow, Koh, Davids, Button, and Rein (2014), Emanuel, Jarus, and Bart (2008), Perreault and French (2016), and van Abswoude, Nuijen, van der Kamp, and Steenbergen (2018) did not find significant differences between performance after external or internal focus of attention instructions measured both during practice (Emanuel et al, 2008;van Abswoude et al, 2018) and during retention test assessed 24-48 h after the last practice session (Chow et al, 2014;Emanuel et al, 2008;Perreault & French, 2016;van Abswoude et al, 2018). On the other hand, many studies replicated the beneficial effects of adopting an external focus of attention as measured during practice (Abdollahipour, Wulf, Psotta, & Nieto, 2015) or following retention test 24-48 h after practice (Brocken, Kal, & van der Kamp, 2016;Flores, Schild, & Chiviacowsky, 2015;Hadler, Chiviacowsky, Wulf, & Schild, 2014;Thorn, 2006).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%