2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.rlp.2015.05.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Diferencias en el comportamiento visual y motor de tenistas en laboratorio y en pista de tenis

Abstract: El estudio compara el comportamiento visual y motor de 40 tenistas noveles en laboratorio (2D) y en pista de tenis (3D) cuando visualizan una secuencia de passing-shots ejecutada por un oponente experto desde el fondo de la pista. Los tenistas deben responder rápida y precisamente mediante un armado de volea de derecha o de revés según la dirección del golpeo. Para el registro del comportamiento visual, se utiliza el sistema tecnológico ASL SE 5000, mientras que para el comportamiento motor, se adapta un siste… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
0
6
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Luis-del Campo et al found that ball-flight trajectory was the visual location with the highest percentage of fixation time when tennis players, located near to the net location, had to simulate a volley for a sequence of video-projected forehand and backhand shots performed by an expert player placed at the backcourt (43.98% for experts and 38.67% for novices of total fixation time) [ 50 ]. Similarly, this ball location was most fixated on by tennis players when returning a passing-shot rally performed by an expert opponent, both in a laboratory setting (62.07% total fixation time) and on a tennis court (58.76% total fixation time) [ 28 ]. Land and McLeod also found that cricket batsmen tracked the ball for between 50% and 80% of the ball trajectory [ 51 ].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Luis-del Campo et al found that ball-flight trajectory was the visual location with the highest percentage of fixation time when tennis players, located near to the net location, had to simulate a volley for a sequence of video-projected forehand and backhand shots performed by an expert player placed at the backcourt (43.98% for experts and 38.67% for novices of total fixation time) [ 50 ]. Similarly, this ball location was most fixated on by tennis players when returning a passing-shot rally performed by an expert opponent, both in a laboratory setting (62.07% total fixation time) and on a tennis court (58.76% total fixation time) [ 28 ]. Land and McLeod also found that cricket batsmen tracked the ball for between 50% and 80% of the ball trajectory [ 51 ].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An exception was Singer et al [ 27 ], who found that expert tennis players with better ranking displayed a smooth tracking of the ball after the serve, compared to lower-ranked players, when collecting visual behaviours of participants in situ (i.e., on the tennis court with movement responses). Luis-del Campo et al [ 28 ] compared the visual and motor behaviours of novice tennis players when observing passing shots performed by an expert tennis player located at the back line of the court, both in laboratory and on court. These tennis players developed a differentiated visual behaviour depending on the experimental setting (i.e., they fixated more on the trunk and hip in laboratory and more on the head and shoulders on a court).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This study intended to prove if the service is a closed skill, as had been normally commented; but it is quite the opposite according to the results, since if it were closed, it could be performed very well by top level competition players as the participants of the study. Campo et al (2015), whilst studying the visual search, observed and classified the vision of the ball into four phases both in lab conditions and court conditions: A) the ball leaving the ball machine to entering the player's field of vision; B) the moment the ball enters the players field of vision to the bounce; C) from the bounce to shot contact; and D) from the contact to the moment the ball leaves the player's field of vision . The fact that the ball becomes the most important spacial location in all phases (except phase D) highlights the role of the gaze in these phases, in which fixation is based around intercepting the ball, according to some studies of different sports (Lebeau et al, 2016).…”
Section: Quantitative Research Related To Ocular Movementmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As a player starts fixing your eyes and your head ("gaze"), they start searching for motor solutions to several challenges at the same time, one is stability at impact, and another is maintaining high concentration at the time of impact (including a just before and after), so as not to lose concentration on other aspects such as the target or the position of the opponent. This loss of concentration can be related to the anxiety or stress generated by the importance of a point (subjective to the player) (Luis del Campo, Reina, Sabido, & Moreno, 2015).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%