1981
DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.17.2.215
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Development of pointing as a social gesture.

Abstract: This study sought to discover the age at which infants call interesting objects to another's attention by pointing, to relate their ability to follow another's pointing to their own use of the gesture, and to compare the uses of pointing and reaching. Infants between 10.5 and 16.5 months of age were studied with their mothers in a setting containing six special stimulus objects. By 12.5 months, a majority of infants pointed, usually vocalizing or looking at their partner while pointing. The communicative funct… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

9
189
4
14

Year Published

1996
1996
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 286 publications
(216 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
(6 reference statements)
9
189
4
14
Order By: Relevance
“…These findings support the position that pointing with the index finger is a milestone in social-cognitive development (Liszkowski & Tomasello, 2011) and the development of conventional communication (Leung & Rheingold, 1981). The index finger shape of pointing thus reflects advances in the child's communication and readiness for successful language acquisition which in turn results in better linguistic skills.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 76%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These findings support the position that pointing with the index finger is a milestone in social-cognitive development (Liszkowski & Tomasello, 2011) and the development of conventional communication (Leung & Rheingold, 1981). The index finger shape of pointing thus reflects advances in the child's communication and readiness for successful language acquisition which in turn results in better linguistic skills.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 76%
“…The predictive value of the index finger shape compared to whole-hand pointing has remained understudied, but there is some indication that index finger pointing involves more complex communicative skills compared to whole-hand pointing. For example, whole-hand pointing emerges earlier in ontogeny (Lock, Young, Service, & Chandler, 1990), presents a nonconventionalized form of communication (Leung & Rheingold, 1981), and is shared with other nonhuman primates (Leavens & Hopkins, 1999). Furthermore, 12-month-olds who point with their index finger-compared to infants who only point with the whole hand-understand communicative intentions better, point more frequently, and accompany their pointing more often by communicative vocalizations (Liszkowski & Tomasello, 2011).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, during this time, the proportion of unreciprocated eye gaze makes up 94.02% of the total amount of the mother's eye gaze directed towards Eva. Although we do expect that with increased mobility Eva starts to explore the environment (Leung & Rheingold, 1981;Adamson, 1995;Swisher, 1992), we also expect the dyad to be able to 'coordinate or systematically divide attention between objects and social partners' which would allow mother and child 'to establish a joint focus on an object or event while also allowing the infant to receive communicative information produced by the mother' (Waxman & Spencer, 1997: 105). However, this is not the case for Eva and Sophia.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Distant pointing is such an fundamental skill that humans acquire it in their early childhood [LR81]. Bolt proposed to use distant pointing for interacting with computing systems, in his seminal work on multimodal interfaces [Bol80].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%