2018
DOI: 10.1111/jtsb.12159
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Contempt and disgust: the emotions of disrespect

Abstract: Contempt and disgust share a number of features which distinguish them from other hostile emotions: they both present two distinct facets—a nonmoral facet and a moral one; they both imply a negative evaluation of the dispositional kind as well as disrespect towards the target of the feeling; and they trigger avoidance and exclusion action tendencies. However, while sharing a common core, contempt and disgust are in our view distinct emotions, qualified by different cognitive‐motivational features. Contempt is … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
16
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 30 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 80 publications
0
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This idea is in line with results from studies demonstrating that when people are not provided with any explicit information, they trust complete strangers or naturally presume that others think the same way they do (Dunning, Anderson, Schlösser, Ehlebracht, & Fetchenhauer, 2014; Ross, Greene, & House, 1977). However, this explanation seems unlikely when a photographed or video-recorded sender expresses an intrinsically nonaffiliative emotion (e.g., anger or contempt; Miceli & Castelfranchi, 2017). Here, the lack of accompanying explicit information may suggest that the sender’s intent is negative (e.g., the receiver may think that a frown or a sneer is directed at him or her), and thus no congruent reaction should occur.…”
Section: The Top-down Control Of Emotional Contagion and Mimicry By Smentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This idea is in line with results from studies demonstrating that when people are not provided with any explicit information, they trust complete strangers or naturally presume that others think the same way they do (Dunning, Anderson, Schlösser, Ehlebracht, & Fetchenhauer, 2014; Ross, Greene, & House, 1977). However, this explanation seems unlikely when a photographed or video-recorded sender expresses an intrinsically nonaffiliative emotion (e.g., anger or contempt; Miceli & Castelfranchi, 2017). Here, the lack of accompanying explicit information may suggest that the sender’s intent is negative (e.g., the receiver may think that a frown or a sneer is directed at him or her), and thus no congruent reaction should occur.…”
Section: The Top-down Control Of Emotional Contagion and Mimicry By Smentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Contempt, in which other people are regarded as beneath some standard (W. I. Miller, 1997), is related to but clearly distinguishable from disgust (Miceli & Castelfranchi, 2018), in which objects or behaviors are considered substandard, and from shame, in which aspects of the self are seen as substandard (S. Miller, 1985; Roseman, 2013).…”
Section: Differentiating Contempt Anger and Disgustmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Gervais & Fessler, 2017). Like disgust, contempt is thought to be a "hostile emotion", however, underpinned by different cognitive-motivational components and social function (for a discussion on features of contempt see, Miceli & Castelfranchi, 2018). The RaFD is highly standardised (no glasses, make-up or facial hair), includes models with modern haircuts and neutral clothing compared to the Ekman-Friesen Pictures of Facial Affect, and takes eye-gaze and camera position into account.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%