1999
DOI: 10.1901/jaba.1999.32-285
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Competition Between Positive and Negative Reinforcement in the Treatment of Escape Behavior

Abstract: We compared the effects of reinforcing compliance with either positive reinforcement (edible items) or negative reinforcement (a break) on 5 participants' escape-maintained problem behavior. Both procedures were assessed with or without extinction. Results showed that compliance was higher and problem behavior was lower for all participants when compliance produced an edible item rather than a break. Treatment gains were achieved without the use of extinction. Results are discussed regarding the use of positiv… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

18
155
4
1

Year Published

2002
2002
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 160 publications
(178 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
(24 reference statements)
18
155
4
1
Order By: Relevance
“…A number of possible interventions exist to attenuate the aversiveness of the original EO, such as poorly presented demands (Carbone, Morgenstern, ZecchinTirri, & Kolberg, 2007;McGill, 1999). These interventions include delivering reinforcement for task compliance (Lalli et al, 1999); embedding demands in the context of preferred activities (Carr et al, 1980); using errorless learning (Ebanks & Fisher, 2003); fading instructions (Pace, Iwata, Cowdery, Andree, & McIntyre, 1993); altering the duration, rate, and novelty of demands (Smith, Iwata, Goh, & Shore, 1995); varying the tasks (McComas, Hoch, Paone, & El-Roy, 2000); and providing a choice of tasks (Dyer, Dunlap, & Winterling, 1990).…”
Section: Figure 2 Depiction Of the Cmo-s Relation As A Results Of Thmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A number of possible interventions exist to attenuate the aversiveness of the original EO, such as poorly presented demands (Carbone, Morgenstern, ZecchinTirri, & Kolberg, 2007;McGill, 1999). These interventions include delivering reinforcement for task compliance (Lalli et al, 1999); embedding demands in the context of preferred activities (Carr et al, 1980); using errorless learning (Ebanks & Fisher, 2003); fading instructions (Pace, Iwata, Cowdery, Andree, & McIntyre, 1993); altering the duration, rate, and novelty of demands (Smith, Iwata, Goh, & Shore, 1995); varying the tasks (McComas, Hoch, Paone, & El-Roy, 2000); and providing a choice of tasks (Dyer, Dunlap, & Winterling, 1990).…”
Section: Figure 2 Depiction Of the Cmo-s Relation As A Results Of Thmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous findings indicate that children who engage in problem behavior maintained by escape from demands may choose a food item over the functional reinforcer during treatment (DeLeon, Neidert, Anders, & Rodriguez-Catter, 2001;Lalli et al, 1999). However, a number of variables may influence choice between concurrently available forms of reinforcement.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A number of possible interventions exist to attenuate the aversiveness of the original EO, such as poorly presented demands (Carbone, Morgenstern, ZecchinTirri, & Kolberg, 2007;McGill, 1999). These interventions include delivering reinforcement for task compliance (Lalli et al, 1999); embedding demands in the context of preferred activities (Carr et al, 1980); using errorless learning (Ebanks & Fisher, 2003); fading instructions (Pace, Iwata, Cowdery, Andree, & McIntyre, 1993); altering the duration, rate, and novelty of demands (Smith, Iwata, Goh, & Shore, 1995); varying the tasks (McComas, Hoch, Paone, & El-Roy, 2000); and providing a choice of tasks (Dyer, Dunlap, & Winterling, 1990).…”
Section: Figure 2 Depiction Of the Cmo-s Relation As A Results Of Thmentioning
confidence: 99%