2003
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.165
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognitive dissonance, double forced compliance, and commitment

Abstract: Double forced compliance situations are studied to analyse how attitudes change after the performance of two behaviours, rather than just one as in standard forced (or induced) compliance situations. In the present experiment, subjects were asked to execute two successive counterattitudinal behaviours: writing an essay in favour of selective admission to the third year of university (first behaviour) and giving a convincing speech in favour of selective admission (second behaviour). The first behaviour was alw… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
16
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 13 publications
1
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…One of the most long-lived expressions of this approach is dissonance theory, advanced by Festinger (1957), who undoubtedly would be astonished to find controversy still attending his classic study with Carlsmith (Festinger & Carlsmith 1959, Harmon-Jones 2000b, and active research on free choice (Kitayama et al 2004), postdecision regret (Brownstein et al 2004), and even selective exposure to (in)consistent data (Jonas et al 2001). Joule & Azdia (2003) have supported their "radical model," which argues for a return to the original version of dissonance theory, and rejects the many alternatives that have developed over the years (also see Beauvois & Joule 1999). Harmon-Jones et al's (2003) action-oriented model, which posits that cognitive discrepancies generate dissonance because they interfere with efficient belief-consistent actions, also is compatible with the fundamental premises of dissonance, as is recent research that demonstrates vicarious dissonance arousal (Monin et al 2004, Norton et al 2003.…”
Section: Dissonancementioning
confidence: 99%
“…One of the most long-lived expressions of this approach is dissonance theory, advanced by Festinger (1957), who undoubtedly would be astonished to find controversy still attending his classic study with Carlsmith (Festinger & Carlsmith 1959, Harmon-Jones 2000b, and active research on free choice (Kitayama et al 2004), postdecision regret (Brownstein et al 2004), and even selective exposure to (in)consistent data (Jonas et al 2001). Joule & Azdia (2003) have supported their "radical model," which argues for a return to the original version of dissonance theory, and rejects the many alternatives that have developed over the years (also see Beauvois & Joule 1999). Harmon-Jones et al's (2003) action-oriented model, which posits that cognitive discrepancies generate dissonance because they interfere with efficient belief-consistent actions, also is compatible with the fundamental premises of dissonance, as is recent research that demonstrates vicarious dissonance arousal (Monin et al 2004, Norton et al 2003.…”
Section: Dissonancementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Since the committing action is very similar to a habitual choice, the effect of commitment may have been weakened. In this regard, to be noted is that Joule and Azdia (2003) proved that additional commitment actions do not always produce a further increase of attitude.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Whenever people publicly take a stance, they also feel a need to maintain their initial choice in order to appear consistent (Cialdini 1993). If people have perceived the initial choice as their own, they feel more compelled to be coherent with their acts (Kiesler and Sakumura 1966;Joule and Azdia 2003). Also the importance and the number of the acts performed by the person influence the level of commitment; but even trivial choices performed only once may create a commitment, as shown by the previous examples.…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%
“…2 Whereas defense motivation could be compelling as an account for non-arbitrary predictions, what about entirely arbitrary predictions? Cognitive dissonance theory and various empirical findings suggest that when a strong external justification for a dissonanceprovoking action is available, the justification is readily used to diffuse or avoid such dissonance (e.g., Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959;Joule & Azdia, 2003). Therefore, a defense motivation account might suggest an absence of selective exposure after arbitrary predictions.…”
Section: Defense Motivationmentioning
confidence: 99%