2009
DOI: 10.3758/pbr.16.6.1037
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognition and native-language grammar: The organizational role of adjective-noun word order in information representation

Abstract: Decades of inquiry have gone into investigating the notion that language shapes human experience, on the premise that the world is highly complicated and that language assists in making it palatable and understandable. In the present work, we address a related, but more specific, proposition: that the various ways in which languages grammatically organize experience lead to different ways of cognitively organizing experience. Whorf's (1956) linguistic relativity principle is the idea that differences across la… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
4
1
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
(11 reference statements)
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Moving beyond adjective order, similar analyses might help explain the cross‐linguistic processing differences that have been observed in languages with postnominal adjective biases (Percy, Sherman, Garcia‐Marques, Mata, & Garcia‐Marques, ; see also Lambert & Paivio, ). More broadly, this characterization might shed light on the existence of strong cross‐linguistic word order preferences for nominal modifiers (Culbertson, Smolensky, & Legendre, ; Greenberg, ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…Moving beyond adjective order, similar analyses might help explain the cross‐linguistic processing differences that have been observed in languages with postnominal adjective biases (Percy, Sherman, Garcia‐Marques, Mata, & Garcia‐Marques, ; see also Lambert & Paivio, ). More broadly, this characterization might shed light on the existence of strong cross‐linguistic word order preferences for nominal modifiers (Culbertson, Smolensky, & Legendre, ; Greenberg, ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…Besides semantic biases, however, it is clear that repeated use of specific syntactic structures may impose specific cognitive challenges to speakers, or foster specific processing habits, which in the long term might enhance specific ways of processing information beyond the linguistic domain. Recent research on the effect of syntax on the processing of events, for instance, has shown (1) an effect of canonical noun-adjective word order on the speed at which noun categories are retrieved 48 , (2) and on recognition memory and similarity judgments while classifying items 49 , as well as (3) an effect of transitive vs. intransitive structures, and agentive vs. non-agentive structures (such as “she broke the vase” vs. “the vase broke itself”), on people’s capacity to remember who was the agent in accidental events 5052 . To our knowledge, however, there is surprisingly no research on the effects of the syntax of one’s native language on cognitive processes unrelated to categorization and discrimination tasks.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When people conceptualize transgender groups in terms of the existing binary model of gender, these emergent groups may also become associated with content that distinguishes them from cisgender groups (Ford & Stangor, 1992;Sherman et al, 2009). Rather than extending stereotypes on the basis of shared gender identity or shared sex assigned at birth, people may consider transgender women and men primarily part of the linguistically marked superordinate category transgender (e.g., Nario-Redmond, 2010;Percy, Sherman, Garcia-Marques, Mata, & Garcia-Marques, 2009). One possibility is that the relative scarcity of transgender individuals, as well as a history of being medically pathologized, could lead to these groups being stereotyped as unusual or deviant in a way that cisgender groups are not (Gazzola & Morrison, 2014;Reed, Franks, & Scherr, 2015;Winter et al, 2009).…”
Section: Stereotyping Emergent Groupsmentioning
confidence: 99%