2019
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0217207
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cake or broccoli? Recency biases children’s verbal responses

Abstract: One of the greatest challenges of developmental psychology is figuring out what children are thinking. This is particularly difficult in early childhood, for children who are prelinguistic or are just beginning to speak their first words. In this stage, children’s responses are commonly measured by presenting young children with a limited choice between one of a small number of options (e.g., “Do you want X or Y?”). A tendency to choose one response in these tasks may be taken as an indication of a child’s pre… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
8
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
2
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The first possibility is that increased efficiency in exploration hinges on changes in a child's core capabilities, such as working memory, which increases over the course of childhood (e.g. [73][74][75]). The increase in working memory could directly result in more efficient exploration.…”
Section: (D) Safe Environments Enable Neophilliamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The first possibility is that increased efficiency in exploration hinges on changes in a child's core capabilities, such as working memory, which increases over the course of childhood (e.g. [73][74][75]). The increase in working memory could directly result in more efficient exploration.…”
Section: (D) Safe Environments Enable Neophilliamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As has been discussed at length elsewhere (Wagenmakers et al, 2018), a Bayes factor (BF) provides the relative weight of the evidence for the null versus the alternative hypothesis, therefore providing the graded strength or reliability for the null hypothesis. A BF 10 of 1 indicates a lack of evidence for either hypothesis, whereas values that increase toward positive infinity indicate increasingly positive evidence for the alternative hypothesis and values that decrease 2 Based on a reviewer's suggestion, we found a slight bias toward higher confidence in the right-hand answer (54%, SD ϭ 0.11, always the second choice; see Sumner, DeAngelis, Hyatt, Goodman, & Kidd, 2019), t(47) ϭ 2.45, p ϭ .018, d ϭ 0.35. It is important to note that this did not differ between the within-and across-domain conditions (53% within, SD ϭ 0.14; 54% across, SD ϭ 0.11), t(47) ϭ 0.72, p ϭ .473, d ϭ 0.10, so we did not include this variable in further analyses.…”
Section: Confidence Comparisonmentioning
confidence: 82%
“…Based on a reviewer’s suggestion, we found a slight bias toward higher confidence in the right-hand answer (54%, SD = 0.11, always the second choice; see Sumner, DeAngelis, Hyatt, Goodman, & Kidd, 2019), t (47) = 2.45, p = .018, d = 0.35. It is important to note that this did not differ between the within- and across-domain conditions (53% within, SD = 0.14; 54% across, SD = 0.11), t (47) = 0.72, p = .473, d = 0.10, so we did not include this variable in further analyses.…”
mentioning
confidence: 92%
“…Even very young children understand that yes–no questions call for a yes or a no (Horgan, 1978) and that forced-choice questions ask one to choose one of the proffered options (Sumner et al, 2019), and this understanding likely contributes to high rates of guessing in response to these kinds of questions (Waterman et al, 2000). By the same token, the results suggest that young children’s recognition that questions about color and number call for a color name or a number, coupled with their ability to generate color names and numbers (Wagner et al, 2013), leads to high rates of guessing in response to color/number questions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%